Comparative estimation systems perform under severely limited workload capacity

Abstract Like many species, humans can perform non-verbal estimates of quantity through our innate approximate number system. However, the cognitive mechanisms that govern how we compare these estimates are not well understood. Little research has addressed how the human estimation-system evaluates multiple quantities, and fewer studies have considered the cost to cognitive workload when undertaking such a task. Here, we provide a novel application of Systems Factorial Technology (SFT; Townsend and Nozawa, 1995) to a comparative estimation task. Across a series of four experiments, we assess whether quantities i.e. non-overlapping red and blue discs, are estimated simultaneously (in parallel) or sequentially (in serial), and under what restrictions to cognitive workload. Our findings reveal that two item-sets may be estimated simultaneously through a parallel estimation system, under severe restrictions to cognitive workload capacity. These restrictions were so severe, as to make the parallel estimation of two quantities less efficient than the estimation of each quantity in succession. While the estimation of a single item-set may be colloquially considered an effortless process, our results show that the estimation of multiple item-sets is a rather demanding feat.

[1]  E. L. Kaufman,et al.  The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.

[2]  H. Egeth,et al.  Parallel versus serial processing in visual search: further evidence from subadditive effects of visual quality. , 1991 .

[3]  Tony Wang,et al.  Understanding the influence of distractors on workload capacity , 2015 .

[4]  G R Grice,et al.  Absence of a redundant-signals effect in a reaction time task with divided attention , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Ami Eidels,et al.  Where similarity beats redundancy: the importance of context, higher order similarity, and response assignment. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  J. Townsend SOME RESULTS CONCERNING THE IDENTIFIABILITY OF PARALLEL AND SERIAL PROCESSES , 1972 .

[7]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  Sally Cochrane,et al.  The Munsell Color System: a scientific compromise from the world of art. , 2014, Studies in history and philosophy of science.

[9]  Richard Schweickert,et al.  A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a stroop task , 1978 .

[10]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[12]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[13]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The statistical properties of the Survivor Interaction Contrast , 2010 .

[14]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[15]  Steven Yantis,et al.  An interactive race model of divided attention. , 1991 .

[16]  Ami Eidels,et al.  How Do Information Processing Systems Deal with Conflicting Information? Differential Predictions for Serial, Parallel, and Coactive Models , 2018 .

[17]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decomposing the reaction time distribution: Pure insertion and selective influence revisited☆ , 1980 .

[18]  M. Lee,et al.  Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical Course , 2014 .

[19]  James T. Townsend,et al.  A note on the identifiability of parallel and serial processes , 1971 .

[20]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Comparing Performance in Discrete and Continuous Comparison Tasks , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  A hierarchical Bayesian approach to distinguishing serial and parallel processing , 2017 .

[22]  James T. Townsend,et al.  Uncovering mental processes with factorial experiments , 1984 .

[23]  J. Townsend,et al.  Workload capacity spaces: A unified methodology for response time measures of efficiency as workload is varied , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  S. Levy,et al.  Counting within the Subitizing Range: The Effect of Number of Distractors on the Perception of Subset Items , 2013, PloS one.

[25]  J. Townsend,et al.  On the costs and benefits of faces and words: process characteristics of feature search in highly meaningful stimuli. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  The precision of mapping between number words and the approximate number system predicts children's formal math abilities. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[27]  Ruth Kimchi,et al.  Figure-Ground Segmentation Can Occur Without Attention , 2008, Psychological science.

[28]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[29]  R. Schweickert,et al.  Selective Influence and Response Time Cumulative Distribution Functions in Serial-Parallel Task Networks. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[30]  Daniel Algom,et al.  A system factorial technology analysis of the size congruity effect: Implications for numerical cognition and stochastic modeling , 2018, Journal of Mathematical Psychology.

[31]  J. Starns,et al.  The Approximate Number System Acuity Redefined: A Diffusion Model Approach , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[32]  Ami Eidels,et al.  Evaluating perceptual integration: uniting response-time- and accuracy-based methodologies , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[33]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[34]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  Nice Guys Finish Fast and Bad Guys Finish Last: Facilitatory vs. Inhibitory Interaction in Parallel Systems. , 2011, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[35]  D. Navon Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  Bayesian Analyses of Cognitive Architecture , 2017, Psychological methods.

[37]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  Systems Factorial Technology provides new insights on global-local information processing in autism spectrum disorders. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[38]  J. Townsend,et al.  NIH Public Access Author Manuscript , 2006 .

[39]  Marco Dadda,et al.  Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitofish , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[40]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .

[41]  J. Townsend,et al.  A theory of interactive parallel processing: new capacity measures and predictions for a response time inequality series. , 2004, Psychological review.

[42]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[43]  E. Dzhafarov Unconditionally Selective Dependence of Random Variables on External Factors. , 2001, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[44]  Daniel Algom,et al.  Comparative judgment of numerosity and numerical magnitude: attention preempts automaticity. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[46]  R. Schweickert,et al.  Response time distributions: Some simple effects of factors selectively influencing mental processes , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[47]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Indexing the approximate number system. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[48]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Multiple Spatially Overlapping Sets Can Be Enumerated in Parallel , 2006, Psychological science.

[49]  J. Townsend,et al.  Stochastic dependencies in parallel and serial models: effects on systems factorial interactions , 1994 .

[50]  Guido Marco Cicchini,et al.  Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[51]  David Premack,et al.  Primative mathematical concepts in the chimpanzee: proportionality and numerosity , 1981, Nature.

[52]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[54]  David C Burr,et al.  Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[55]  I. Pepperberg,et al.  Number comprehension by a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), including a zero-like concept. , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[56]  Paul B. Buckley,et al.  Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.