Comparative estimation systems perform under severely limited workload capacity
暂无分享,去创建一个
Joseph W. Houpt | Ami Eidels | David Landy | Zachary Howard | Zachary L Howard | Paul Garrett | Zachary L. Howard | A. Eidels | D. Landy | P. Garrett
[1] E. L. Kaufman,et al. The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.
[2] H. Egeth,et al. Parallel versus serial processing in visual search: further evidence from subadditive effects of visual quality. , 1991 .
[3] Tony Wang,et al. Understanding the influence of distractors on workload capacity , 2015 .
[4] G R Grice,et al. Absence of a redundant-signals effect in a reaction time task with divided attention , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[5] Ami Eidels,et al. Where similarity beats redundancy: the importance of context, higher order similarity, and response assignment. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] J. Townsend. SOME RESULTS CONCERNING THE IDENTIFIABILITY OF PARALLEL AND SERIAL PROCESSES , 1972 .
[7] Jeff Miller,et al. Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] Sally Cochrane,et al. The Munsell Color System: a scientific compromise from the world of art. , 2014, Studies in history and philosophy of science.
[9] Richard Schweickert,et al. A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a stroop task , 1978 .
[10] Rochel Gelman,et al. Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[11] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[12] Philippe Pinel,et al. Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.
[13] James T. Townsend,et al. The statistical properties of the Survivor Interaction Contrast , 2010 .
[14] C. Gallistel,et al. Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.
[15] Steven Yantis,et al. An interactive race model of divided attention. , 1991 .
[16] Ami Eidels,et al. How Do Information Processing Systems Deal with Conflicting Information? Differential Predictions for Serial, Parallel, and Coactive Models , 2018 .
[17] J. Townsend,et al. Decomposing the reaction time distribution: Pure insertion and selective influence revisited☆ , 1980 .
[18] M. Lee,et al. Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical Course , 2014 .
[19] James T. Townsend,et al. A note on the identifiability of parallel and serial processes , 1971 .
[20] Tali Leibovich,et al. Comparing Performance in Discrete and Continuous Comparison Tasks , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[21] Joseph W. Houpt,et al. A hierarchical Bayesian approach to distinguishing serial and parallel processing , 2017 .
[22] James T. Townsend,et al. Uncovering mental processes with factorial experiments , 1984 .
[23] J. Townsend,et al. Workload capacity spaces: A unified methodology for response time measures of efficiency as workload is varied , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[24] S. Levy,et al. Counting within the Subitizing Range: The Effect of Number of Distractors on the Perception of Subset Items , 2013, PloS one.
[25] J. Townsend,et al. On the costs and benefits of faces and words: process characteristics of feature search in highly meaningful stimuli. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. The precision of mapping between number words and the approximate number system predicts children's formal math abilities. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[27] Ruth Kimchi,et al. Figure-Ground Segmentation Can Occur Without Attention , 2008, Psychological science.
[28] S. Dehaene,et al. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .
[29] R. Schweickert,et al. Selective Influence and Response Time Cumulative Distribution Functions in Serial-Parallel Task Networks. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[30] Daniel Algom,et al. A system factorial technology analysis of the size congruity effect: Implications for numerical cognition and stochastic modeling , 2018, Journal of Mathematical Psychology.
[31] J. Starns,et al. The Approximate Number System Acuity Redefined: A Diffusion Model Approach , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[32] Ami Eidels,et al. Evaluating perceptual integration: uniting response-time- and accuracy-based methodologies , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[33] C. Gallistel,et al. Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .
[34] Joseph W. Houpt,et al. Nice Guys Finish Fast and Bad Guys Finish Last: Facilitatory vs. Inhibitory Interaction in Parallel Systems. , 2011, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[35] D. Navon. Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.
[36] Joseph W. Houpt,et al. Bayesian Analyses of Cognitive Architecture , 2017, Psychological methods.
[37] Joseph W. Houpt,et al. Systems Factorial Technology provides new insights on global-local information processing in autism spectrum disorders. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[38] J. Townsend,et al. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript , 2006 .
[39] Marco Dadda,et al. Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitofish , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[40] James T. Townsend,et al. The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .
[41] J. Townsend,et al. A theory of interactive parallel processing: new capacity measures and predictions for a response time inequality series. , 2004, Psychological review.
[42] Saul Sternberg,et al. The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .
[43] E. Dzhafarov. Unconditionally Selective Dependence of Random Variables on External Factors. , 2001, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[44] Daniel Algom,et al. Comparative judgment of numerosity and numerical magnitude: attention preempts automaticity. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[45] Daniel Ansari,et al. Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[46] R. Schweickert,et al. Response time distributions: Some simple effects of factors selectively influencing mental processes , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[47] Matthew Inglis,et al. Indexing the approximate number system. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[48] L. Feigenson,et al. Multiple Spatially Overlapping Sets Can Be Enumerated in Parallel , 2006, Psychological science.
[49] J. Townsend,et al. Stochastic dependencies in parallel and serial models: effects on systems factorial interactions , 1994 .
[50] Guido Marco Cicchini,et al. Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[51] David Premack,et al. Primative mathematical concepts in the chimpanzee: proportionality and numerosity , 1981, Nature.
[52] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[53] Scott D. Brown,et al. The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.
[54] David C Burr,et al. Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[55] I. Pepperberg,et al. Number comprehension by a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), including a zero-like concept. , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.
[56] Paul B. Buckley,et al. Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.