Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories

Abstract We have previously demonstrated that, in preparing themselves to aim voluntary impulses of isometric elbow force to unpredictable targets, subjects selected default values for amplitude and direction according the range of targets that they expected. Once a specific target appeared, subjects specified amplitude and direction through parallel processes. Amplitude was specified continuously from an average or central default; direction was specified stochastically from one of the target directions. Using the same timed response paradigm, we now report three experiments to examine how the time available for processing target information influences trajectory characteristics in two-degree-of-freedom forces and multijoint movements. We first sought to determine whether the specification of force direction could also take the form of a discrete stochastic process in pulses of wrist muscle force, where direction can vary continuously. With four equiprobable targets (two force amplitudes in each of two directions separated by 22° or 90°), amplitude was specified from a central default value for both narrow and wide target separations as a continuous variable. Direction, however, remained specified as a discrete variable for wide target separations. For narrow target separations, the directional distribution of default responses suggested the presence of both discrete and central values. We next examined point-to-point movements in a multijoint planar hand movement task with targets at two distances and two directions but at five directional separations (from 30° to 150° separation). We found that extent was again specified continuously from a central default. Direction was specified discretely from alternative default directions when target separation was wide and continuously from a central default when separation was narrow. The specification of both extent and direction evolved over a 200-ms time period beginning about 100 ms after target presentation. As in elbow force pulses, extent was specified progressively in both correct and wrong direction responses through a progressive improvement in the scaling of acceleration and velocity peaks to the target. On the other hand, movement time and hand path straightness did not change significantly in the course of specification. Thus, the specification of movement time and linearity, global features of the trajectories, are given priority over the specific values of extent and direction. In a third experiment, we varied the distances between unidirectional target pairs and found that movement extent is specified discretely, like direction, when the disparity in distances is large. The implications of these findings for contextual effects on trajectory planning are discussed. The independence of extent and direction specification and the prior setting of response duration and straightness provide critical support for the hypothesis that point-to-point movements are planned vectorially.

[1]  A. Georgopoulos Neural integration of movement: role of motor cortex in reaching , 1988, FASEB journal : official publication of the Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology.

[2]  F. Lacquaniti Central representations of human limb movement as revealed by studies of drawing and handwriting , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  A. Riehle,et al.  Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Practice effects on the preprogramming of discrete movements in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[5]  G E Alexander,et al.  Movement-related neuronal activity selectively coding either direction or muscle pattern in three motor areas of the monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  D. Meyer,et al.  Models for the speed and accuracy of aimed movements. , 1982, Psychological review.

[7]  A. Riehle Visually induced signal-locked neuronal activity changes in precentral motor areas of the monkey: hierarchical progression of signal processing , 1991, Brain Research.

[8]  Edward V. Evarts,et al.  Electromyography in CNS Disorders , 1985, Neurology.

[9]  B. C. Griffith,et al.  The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  P. Viviani,et al.  The law relating the kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[11]  C. Marsden,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[13]  S. Keele,et al.  Motor Disorder and the Timing of Repetitive Movements a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[14]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Preparation for movement: neural representations of intended direction in three motor areas of the monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  F. Ottes,et al.  Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: Two different modes , 1984, Vision Research.

[16]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Speed—Accuracy Tradeoffs in Aimed Movements: Toward a Theory of Rapid Voluntary Action , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[17]  C. Marsden,et al.  Dual task performance and processing resources in normal subjects and patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  P. Viviani,et al.  The relation between linear extent and velocity in drawing movements , 1983, Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Gordon,et al.  Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  C Ghez,et al.  Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[23]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Control of arm movements in a 2-dimensional pointing task , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  J. Kalaska The representation of arm movements in postcentral and parietal cortex. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[25]  C Ghez,et al.  Proprioceptive control of interjoint coordination. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[26]  John M. Hollerbach,et al.  Workspace Effect in Arm Movement Kinematics Derived by Joint Interpolation , 1987 .

[27]  W. Cleveland Robust Locally Weighted Regression and Smoothing Scatterplots , 1979 .

[28]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Simultaneous timing of multiple intervals: implications of the scalar property. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[29]  G E Alexander,et al.  Neural representations of the target (goal) of visually guided arm movements in three motor areas of the monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  J. Tanji,et al.  Reflex and intended responses in motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons of monkey. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  D. Vicario,et al.  The control of rapid limb movement in the cat II. Scaling of isometric force adjustments , 1978, Experimental brain research.

[33]  A P Georgopoulos Neural coding of the direction of reaching and a comparison with saccadic eye movements. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[34]  Philip Bard,et al.  Cognitive spatial-motor processes 2. Information transmitted by the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and by neuronal populations in primate motor cortex and area 5 , 1988 .

[35]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  A. Liberman,et al.  The discrimination of relative onset-time of the components of certain speech and nonspeech patterns. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[37]  R Porter,et al.  The discharges during movement of cells in the ventrolateral thalamus of the conscious monkey. , 1980, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  H. Ross,et al.  Learning "what" and "how" in a human motor task. , 1995, Learning & memory.

[39]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Human arm trajectory formation. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  W A MacKay,et al.  CNV, stretch reflex and reaction time correlates of preparation for movement direction and force. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[41]  H. Yumiya,et al.  Sensory motor processing of target movements in motor cortex. , 1983, Advances in neurology.

[42]  C. Ghez,et al.  Programming of extent and direction in human reaching movements , 1993 .

[43]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[44]  Michel Desmurget,et al.  The effect of viewing the static hand prior to movement onset on pointing kinematics and variability , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  Motor cortical activity in a context-recall task. , 1995, Science.

[46]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[47]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  Spatial coding of visually guided arm movements in primate motor cortex. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[48]  Beat Kleiner,et al.  Graphical Methods for Data Analysis , 1983 .

[49]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Contributions of vision and proprioception to accuracy in limb movements , 1994 .

[50]  M. V. Rhoades,et al.  On the Reduction of Choice Reaction Times with Practice , 1959 .

[51]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.