Counting in Chinese: Cultural variation in a basic cognitive skill

Learning to count is an early and important cognitive accomplishment implicated in the development of later mathematical abilities. Research on the development of counting (e.g., Gelman & Gallistel, 1978 ) has focused on the way that children's understanding of number guides the task of learning to count. Because counting requires use of a number system, the features of a culture's particular numbering system could also impact the development of counting. The current research compared the developmental course of counting in two languages (English & Chinese) with somewhat divergent systems for forming number names. Large country differences generally favoring the Chinese subjects were found in children's ability to form proper number names and in the kinds of errors they make. The “teens” in English pose a particular stumbling block for American children's counting. Common sources of difficulty in counting across cultures were also found. In particular, children in both countries had difficulty with the need to coordinate incrementing units and decades lists in counting. In using counting as a tool to determine how many objects there are in a set, similar country differences were found. However, country effects in counting objects were found only for number-naming errors. Despite the difficulties American children have with mastering the English numbering system, there was no effect of country on the likelihood that children would skip or double count objects. Implications of these results for linguistic models of number generation for theories of the organization of complex skills and for later achievement differences in mathematics are discussed.

[1]  Alex Cherry Wilkinson,et al.  Children's partial knowledge of the cognitive skill of counting , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  The Child's Representation of Number: A Multidimensional Scaling Analysis. , 1983 .

[3]  R. Case,et al.  Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span , 1982 .

[4]  R. K. Young,et al.  Heterogeneity and Skewness in Analysis of Variance , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.

[5]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Child's Understanding of Number , 1979 .

[6]  G. Groen,et al.  A chronometric analysis of simple addition. , 1972 .

[7]  Geoffrey B. Saxe,et al.  Body parts as numerals: A developmental analysis of numeration among the Oksapmin in Papua New Guinea. , 1981 .

[8]  M. Perlmutter,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: comparison of operations. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  Giyoo Hatano,et al.  Performance of expert abacus operators , 1977, Cognition.

[10]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Cognitive processes in verbal-number production: inferences from the performance of brain-damaged subjects. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  J. Stigler,et al.  Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children. , 1986, Science.

[12]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgments of and by Representativeness , 1981 .

[13]  W. G. Cochran The comparison of percentages in matched samples. , 1950, Biometrika.

[14]  C. Brainerd Working-Memory Systems and Cognitive Development , 1983 .

[15]  J. Stigler “Mental abacus”: The effect of abacus training on Chinese children's mental calculation , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  G. Box Some Theorems on Quadratic Forms Applied in the Study of Analysis of Variance Problems, II. Effects of Inequality of Variance and of Correlation Between Errors in the Two-Way Classification , 1954 .

[17]  R. Gelman,et al.  Preschoolers' counting: Principles before skill , 1983, Cognition.

[18]  T. Holyoke,et al.  Number Words and Number Symbols , 1978 .

[19]  T. Gladwin East Is a Big Bird: Navigation and Logic on Puluwat Atoll , 1972 .

[20]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Understandings , 1982 .

[21]  G. Box Some Theorems on Quadratic Forms Applied in the Study of Analysis of Variance Problems, I. Effect of Inequality of Variance in the One-Way Classification , 1954 .

[22]  James W. Stigler,et al.  Consequences of Skill: The Case of Abacus Training in Taiwan , 1986, American Journal of Education.

[23]  R. Gelman,et al.  Young children's numerical competence ** , 1986 .

[24]  Guy J. Groen,et al.  Can Preschool Children Invent Addition Algorithms , 1977 .

[25]  D. Lancy Cross-cultural studies in cognition and mathematics , 1984 .

[26]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: Evidence from dyscalculia , 1985, Brain and Cognition.

[27]  John Richards,et al.  The Acquisition and Elaboration of the Number Word Sequence , 1982 .

[28]  J. Piaget The Child's Conception of Number , 1953 .

[29]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition. , 1978 .

[30]  C. A. Boneau,et al.  The effects of violations of assumptions underlying the test. , 1960, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  A featural analysis of preschoolers' counting knowledge. , 1984 .

[32]  M. Potter,et al.  Spatial enumeration without counting. , 1968, Child development.

[33]  James R. Hurford,et al.  The linguistic theory of numerals , 1975 .

[34]  E. Hutchins Culture and Inference: A Trobriand Case Study , 1980 .

[35]  Georges Ifrah,et al.  From One To Zero , 1985 .