Translation-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states
暂无分享,去创建一个
Tamar H Gollan | Victor S Ferreira | Cynthia Cera | Susanna Flett | V. Ferreira | T. Gollan | S. Flett | Cynthia Cera
[1] Alan S. Brown,et al. From tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) data to theoretical implications in two steps: when more TOTs means better retrieval. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[2] Albert Costa,et al. A further look at semantic context effects in language production: The role of response congruency , 2006 .
[3] T. Gollan,et al. What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish-English and tagalog-English bilinguals. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[4] Albert Costa,et al. Another Look at Cross-Language Competition in Bilingual Speech Production: Lexical and Phonological Factors. , 2003 .
[5] Wido La Heij,et al. Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: Implications for models of lexical access in language production , 2003 .
[6] P. Dixon. Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs , 2008 .
[7] Albert Costa,et al. Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[8] Alissa Melinger,et al. Dismissing lexical competition does not make speaking any easier: A rejoinder to Mahon and Caramazza (2009) , 2009 .
[9] Gerard Kempen,et al. The lexicalization process in sentence production and naming: indirect election of words , 1983, Cognition.
[10] R. Schreuder,et al. Producing words in a foreign language: Can speakers prevent interference from their first language? , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[11] L. Abrams,et al. Why a superman cannot help a tsunami: activation of grammatical class influences resolution of young and older adults' tip-of-the-tongue states. , 2007, Psychology and aging.
[12] Roger S. Brown,et al. The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .
[13] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. The cumulative semantic cost does not reflect lexical selection by competition. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[14] Christine Fennema-Notestine,et al. Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[15] Gregory V. Jones,et al. Back to Woodworth: Role of interlopers in the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon , 1989, Memory & cognition.
[16] D. Burke,et al. Phonological priming effects on word retrieval and tip-of-the-tongue experiences in young and older adults. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[17] Susan C. Bobb,et al. Language selection in bilingual speech: evidence for inhibitory processes. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[18] Albert Costa,et al. On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[19] M. Coltheart,et al. Information Retrieval in Tip of the Tongue States: New Data and Methodological Advances , 2008, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[20] Karen Emmorey,et al. Bimodal bilinguals reveal the source of tip-of-the-tongue states , 2009, Cognition.
[21] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] A Koriat,et al. What does a person in a “TOT” state know that a person in a “don’t know” state doesn’t know , 1974, Memory & cognition.
[23] M. Vitkovitch,et al. Inhibitory effects during object name retrieval: The effect of interval between prime and target on picture naming responses. , 2001, British journal of psychology.
[24] R. Hartsuiker,et al. Does Bilingualism Change Native-Language Reading? , 2009, Psychological science.
[25] T. Dijkstra,et al. Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[26] Albert Costa,et al. Lexical Selection in Bilinguals: Do Words in the Bilingual's Two Lexicons Compete for Selection? , 1999 .
[27] T. Gollan,et al. Proper names get stuck on bilingual and monolingual speakers' tip of the tongue equally often. , 2005, Neuropsychology.
[28] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[29] S. Monsell,et al. Inhibition of Spoken Word Production by Priming a Semantic Competitor , 1994 .
[30] Albert Costa,et al. Assessing the presence of lexical competition across languages: Evidence from the Stroop task* , 2008, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[31] Paul C. Amrhein,et al. Phonological facilitation through translation in a bilingual picture-naming task , 2007, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[32] A Caramazza,et al. Retrieval of lexical-syntactic features in tip-of-the-tongue states. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] Gregory V. Jones,et al. Phonological blocking in the tip of the tongue state , 1987, Cognition.
[34] W. Heij,et al. Components of Stroop-like interference in picture naming , 1988, Memory & cognition.
[35] N. Askari. Priming Effects on Tip-of-the-Tongue States in Farsi-English Bilinguals , 1999 .
[36] T. Jaeger,et al. Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[37] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Semantic interference in a delayed naming task: evidence for the response exclusion hypothesis. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[38] Matthew Flatt,et al. PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .
[39] K. Rayner,et al. Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: the frequency-lag hypothesis. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[40] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the `tip-of-the-tongue' phenomenon , 1997, Cognition.
[41] N. Silverberg,et al. Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew–English bilinguals , 2001, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[42] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[43] T. Sandoval,et al. More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[44] J. Hanley,et al. The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Do experimenter-presented interlopers have any effect? , 1992, Cognition.
[45] K Bock,et al. The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation? , 1992, Memory & cognition.
[46] L. Abrams,et al. Does priming specific syllables during tip-of-the-tongue states facilitate word retrieval in older adults? , 2002, Psychology and aging.
[47] Alissa Melinger,et al. Semantic context effects in language production: A swinging lexical network proposal and a review , 2009 .
[48] Lise Abrams,et al. Syntactic class influences phonological priming of tip-of-the-tongue resolution , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.