Perceiving minimal distinctions in ASL under normal and point-light display conditions

Perceptual confusions within 36 formationally minimal pairs of signs were assessed for native signers under two conditions of video presentation: (1) normally lighted black-and-white displays and (2) point-light displays constructed by affixing 26 points of retroreflective tape on the fingertips, back knuckles, and wrists and 1 point on the nose of the signer. Nine pairs were selected for each of the formational parameters of handshape, location, movement, and orientation. For each minimal pair, a native signer constructed and signed an ASL sentence that was syntactically and semantically appropriate for both members of the pair. Fourteen highly fluent ASL users responded by selecting a picture appropriate to the viewed sentence, thus avoiding contamination by English. For both viewing conditions, subjects discriminated the minimal pairs significantly better than chance. Performance was better (1) when location or orientation differentiated the signs than when movement or handshape did and (2) in normal lighting than in the point-light displays. With the point/light displays, discrimination of location, movement, and orientation was poorer and handshape discrimination was at chance. The discussion considers (1) the confusion of signs in the absence of linguistic redundancy, (2) the effectivenese of the point-light configurations as “minimal cues” for the contrasts, and (3) the efficacy of using these point-light configurations to reduce the information in ASL to a narrow bandwidth.

[1]  W. Stokoe,et al.  A dictionary of American sign language on linguistic principles , 1965 .

[2]  E. Klima The signs of language , 1979 .

[3]  J L Danhauer,et al.  Perceptual features of the manual alphabet. , 1978, American annals of the deaf.

[4]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[5]  J. Cutting,et al.  Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .

[6]  Kenneth C. Knowlton,et al.  Perception of sign language from an array of 27 moving spots , 1981, Nature.

[7]  R. Battison,et al.  Lexical Borrowing in American Sign Language , 1978 .

[8]  R. Battison,et al.  Phonological Deletion in American Sign Language , 2013 .

[9]  J. Douglas Carroll,et al.  Chapter 13 – APPLICATIONS OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES SCALING TO STUDIES OF HUMAN PERCEPTION AND JUDGMENT , 1974 .

[10]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Perception of American sign language in dynamic point-light displays. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Preliminaries to a distinctive feature analysis of handshapes in American Sign Language , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[13]  F. Grosjean Sign & Word Recognition: A First Comparison , 2013 .

[14]  G. A. Miller,et al.  An Analysis of Perceptual Confusions Among Some English Consonants , 1955 .

[15]  J Stungis,et al.  Identification and discrimination of handshape in American Sign Language , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.