A three alternative tracking paradigm to measure vernier acuity of older infants

Vernier acuity was measured in infants 1 to 14 months of age using a dynamic three alternative tracking paradigm. The location of the vernier offset would move randomly to one of three screen locations. An observer, unaware of the stimulus location, viewed the infant from behind a screen and guessed the position of the vernier offset. The magnitude of the vernier offset was controlled by a staircase and the results were analyzed using a broken line psychometric function. These thresholds were compared to thresholds obtained from the same infants during the same session with a two alternative forced choice preferential looking paradigm. Differences in the results of these two procedures and those reported previously in the literature are discussed in terms of the differences in the nature of the visual stimuli, one containing motion and one without motion.

[1]  R Held,et al.  Preferential Looking Acuity in Infants from Two to Fifty‐Eight Weeks of Age , 1980, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[2]  R E Manny,et al.  The development of vernier acuity in infants. , 1984, Current eye research.

[3]  S. McKee,et al.  Statistical properties of forced-choice psychometric functions: Implications of probit analysis , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  G Westheimer,et al.  The spatial sense of the eye. Proctor lecture. , 1979, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[5]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Development of vernier acuity in infants , 1984, Vision Research.

[6]  D. Teller,et al.  Visual acuity for vertical and diagonal gratings in human infants. , 1974, Vision research.

[7]  D. Mayer,et al.  Visual acuity development in infants and young children, as assessed by operant preferential looking , 1982, Vision Research.

[8]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A revised table of d’ for M-alternative forced choice , 1979 .

[9]  S. Klein,et al.  Hyperacuity thresholds of 1 sec: theoretical predictions and empirical validation. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[10]  Velma Dobson,et al.  Visual acuity in human infants: A review and comparison of behavioral and electrophysiological studies , 1978, Vision Research.

[11]  V. Dobson,et al.  Visual acuity in infants--the effect of variations in stimulus luminance within the photopic range. , 1983, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[12]  V. Dreyer,et al.  Visual acuity. , 1974, Ophthalmologica. Journal international d'ophtalmologie. International journal of ophthalmology. Zeitschrift fur Augenheilkunde.

[13]  Suzanne P. McKee,et al.  Integration regions for visual hyperacuity , 1977, Vision Research.

[14]  Gerald Westheimer The oscilloscopic view: Retinal illuminance and contrast of point and line targets , 1985, Vision Research.

[15]  B. R. Shelton,et al.  Two-alternative versus three-alternative procedures for threshold estimation , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  D. Teller The forced-choice preferential looking procedure: A psychophysical technique for use with human infants. , 1979 .

[17]  D M Levi,et al.  Differences in vernier discrimination for grating between strabismic and anisometropic amblyopes. , 1982, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[18]  A Pentland,et al.  Maximum likelihood estimation: The best PEST , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.