Perturbation of perceptual units reveals dominance hierarchy in cross calibration.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Brett R Fajen,et al. Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a collision. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] Felice L. Bedford,et al. Constraints on learning new mappings between perceptual dimensions , 1989 .
[3] M. Landy,et al. Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.
[4] M. Landy,et al. Weighted linear cue combination with possibly correlated error , 2003, Vision Research.
[5] Brett R. Fajen. The scaling of information to action in visually guided braking. , 2005 .
[6] James A Crowell,et al. Distortions of distance and shape are not produced by a single continuous transformation of reach space , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[7] P. Viviani,et al. Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] James A. Balliett,et al. What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. , 1990 .
[9] M S Landy,et al. Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[10] K. Davids,et al. Timing a one-handed catch , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[11] L. S. Mark,et al. Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] F. Domini,et al. Integration of disparity and velocity information for haptic and perceptual judgments of object depth. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[13] Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al. Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and Dynamic Binocular Vision , 2005 .
[14] F. Domini,et al. 3-D structure perceived from dynamic information: a new theory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[15] Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al. Monocular Egocentric Distance Information Generated by Head Movement , 1994 .
[16] David Carl Ipsen. Units, dimensions, and dimensionless numbers , 2011 .
[17] Jacques Droulez,et al. Absolute distance perception during in-depth head movement: calibrating optic flow with extra-retinal information , 2002, Vision Research.
[18] Anne E. Garing,et al. Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Geoffrey P Bingham,et al. Calibrating reach distance to visual targets. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] R. Jacobs. What determines visual cue reliability? , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] Dieterich J. Schuring,et al. Scale Models in Engineering: Fundamentals and Applications , 1977 .
[22] Daniel M Wolpert,et al. Role of uncertainty in sensorimotor control. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[23] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[24] Fulvio Domini,et al. Stereo and motion information are not independently processed by the visual system , 2006, Vision Research.
[25] W. Warren,et al. Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] M. Ernst,et al. The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[27] J M Loomis,et al. Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[28] G P Bingham,et al. Distortions in definite distance and shape perception as measured by reaching without and with haptic feedback. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] Johan Wagemans,et al. Texture and haptic cues in slant discrimination: reliability-based cue weighting without statistically optimal cue combination. , 2005, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[30] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Sensory Cue Integration , 2011 .
[31] G P Bingham,et al. Accommodation, occlusion, and disparity matching are used to guide reaching: a comparison of actual versus virtual environments. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] Daniel S. Mcconnell,et al. Reaching measures of monocular distance perception: Forward versus side-to-side head movements and haptic feedback , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] Michael T. Turvey,et al. Contrasting orientations to the theory of visual information processing. , 1977 .
[34] J. Wann,et al. Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] L S Mark,et al. A biodynamic basis for perceived categories of action: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.
[36] R. Jacobs,et al. Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth , 1999, Vision Research.
[37] W. J. Duncan. Physical Similarity and Dimensional Analysis , 1953 .
[38] Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al. Dynamics and the problem of visual event recognition , 1996 .
[39] Robert Rosen,et al. Fundamentals Of Measurement And Representation Of Natural Systems , 1978 .
[40] W. E. Baker,et al. Similarity Methods in Engineering Dynamics: Theory and Practice of Scale Modeling , 1973 .
[41] Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al. Calibrating grasp size and reach distance: interactions reveal integral organization of reaching-to-grasp movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] J. Droulez,et al. Contribution of extraretinal signals to the scaling of object distance during self-motion , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[43] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[44] G. Bingham,et al. A solution to the online guidance problem for targeted reaches: proportional rate control using relative disparity τ , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[45] Christopher C. Pagano,et al. Comparing measures of monocular distance perception: Verbal and reaching errors are not correlated. , 1998 .
[46] G P Bingham,et al. The necessity of a perception-action approach to definite distance perception: monocular distance perception to guide reaching. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[47] Rob Withagen,et al. Transfer of calibration in length perception by dynamic touch , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[48] James M. Hillis,et al. Slant from texture and disparity cues: optimal cue combination. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[49] William H Warren,et al. The Direction of Walking—but Not Throwing or Kicking—Is Adapted by Optic Flow , 2010, Psychological science.
[50] M. Landy,et al. A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues , 1993, Vision Research.
[51] Fulvio Domini,et al. Combining Image Signals before Three-Dimensional Reconstruction: The Intrinsic Constraint Model of Cue Integration , 2011 .
[52] Rob Withagen,et al. The Calibration of Walking Transfers to Crawling: Are Action Systems Calibrated? , 2002 .
[53] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. The Visual Pathways Mediating Perception and Prehension , 1996 .
[54] J. Saunders,et al. Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant? , 2003, Vision Research.
[55] Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al. The rate of adaptation to displacement prisms remains constant despite acquisition of rapid calibration , 1999 .
[56] Hugo Bruggeman,et al. Learning to throw on a rotating carousel: recalibration based on limb dynamics and projectile kinematics , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[57] Laura F. Fox,et al. Self-motion perception during locomotor recalibration: more than meets the eye. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[58] E. Pedhazur. Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research: Explanation and Prediction , 1982 .
[59] Rob Withagen,et al. The role of feedback information for calibration and attunement in perceiving length by dynamic touch. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[60] Mark Mon-Williams,et al. Natural prehension in trials without haptic feedback but only when calibration is allowed , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[61] David M Jacobs,et al. Lateral interception I: operative optical variables, attunement, and calibration. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[62] M. Landy,et al. Ideal-Observer Models of Cue Integration , 2012 .