Impulsivity and risk-taking behavior in focal frontal lobe lesions

Frontal lobe dysfunction may underlie excessively impulsive and risky behavior observed in a range of neurological disorders. We devised a gambling task to examine these behavior tendencies in a sample of patients who had sustained focal damage to the frontal lobes or nonfrontal cortical regions as well as in a matched sample of healthy control subjects. The main objectives of the study were: (1) to behaviorally dissociate impulsivity and risk-taking; (2) to examine potential associations between specific frontal lesion sites and impulsivity or risk-taking; (3) to investigate the influence of reinforcement and trial timing on both behaviors. Our results indicated that patients and controls were equally likely to perform impulsively. Risk-taking performance strategies, however, were related to left ventrolateral and orbital lesion sites. Moreover, risk-taking was also associated with blunted response alteration following a nonrewarded trial. Patients and control subjects showed identical responses to reward-timing manipulations consistent with formal decision-making theory. These findings suggest that ventrolateral and orbital lesions are related to the reward-based aspects of decision-making (risk-taking) rather than to simple response disinhibition (impulsivity). Reduced reaction to the negative consequences of one's actions may underlie this behavior pattern.

[1]  E. Rolls The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  M. Farah,et al.  Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  M. Farah,et al.  Dissociable elements of human foresight: a role for the ventromedial frontal lobes in framing the future, but not in discounting future rewards , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  F. Lhermitte 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  A contribution to the anatomical basis of thalamic amnesia. , 1985, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  Peter H. M. P. Roelofsma,et al.  Anomalies in intertemporal choice , 1996 .

[7]  Peter B Barker,et al.  Anatomy of Spatial Attention: Insights from Perfusion Imaging and Hemispatial Neglect in Acute Stroke , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Robert A. Bornstein,et al.  The Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised , 1990 .

[9]  W. Hulstijn,et al.  Impulsivity in abstinent early- and late-onset alcoholics: differences in self-report measures and a discounting task. , 2006, Addiction.

[10]  D. Stuss,et al.  Organizational strategies with unilateral or bilateral frontal lobe injury in word learning tasks. , 1994 .

[11]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[12]  T. Shallice,et al.  Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  E T Rolls,et al.  Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  M. Ullsperger Performance monitoring in neurological and psychiatric patients. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[15]  D. Stuss,et al.  Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Deficits in decision-making in patients with aneurysms of the anterior communicating artery. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  J. Busemeyer,et al.  A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task. , 2002, Psychological assessment.

[19]  L. Cushman,et al.  Neuroanatomic and neuropsychological predictors of unawareness of cognitive deficit in the vascular population , 1994 .

[20]  T Shallice,et al.  Impaired concentration due to frontal lobe damage from two distinct lesion sites , 2005, Neurology.

[21]  R. J. Dolan,et al.  Differential neural response to positive and negative feedback in planning and guessing tasks , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  C. Reynolds,et al.  Wechsler memory scale-revised , 1988 .

[24]  J. Patton,et al.  Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. , 1995, Journal of clinical psychology.

[25]  R. Barkley,et al.  Executive Functioning, Temporal Discounting, and Sense of Time in Adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) , 2001, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[26]  Luke Clark,et al.  The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  L. R. Peterson,et al.  Short-term retention of individual verbal items. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Asymmetric Functional Roles of Right and Left Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortices in Social Conduct, Decision-Making, and Emotional Processing , 2002, Cortex.

[29]  H. Garavan,et al.  Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[30]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[31]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Does gender play a role in functional asymmetry of ventromedial prefrontal cortex? , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Investment Behavior and the Negative Side of Emotion , 2005, Psychological science.

[33]  D. Stuss,et al.  California Verbal Learning Test: performance by patients with focal frontal and non-frontal lesions. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choices, Values, and Frames , 2000 .

[35]  John Brown Some Tests of the Decay Theory of Immediate Memory , 1958 .

[36]  A. Beck,et al.  Screening depressed patients in family practice. A rapid technic. , 1972, Postgraduate medicine.

[37]  R. R. Abidin Psychological Assessment Resources , 1995 .

[38]  J. Horne,et al.  A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. , 1976, International journal of chronobiology.

[39]  Laurie A. Miller,et al.  Impulsivity, risk-taking, and the ability to synthesize fragmented information after frontal lobectomy , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  D. Pandya,et al.  Association pathways of the prefrontal cortex and functional observations , 2002 .

[41]  D. Stuss,et al.  Principles of frontal lobe function , 2002 .

[42]  M. Petrides Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and the macaque frontal cortex , 1994 .

[43]  D. Stuss,et al.  Evidence from Focal Lesions in Humans , 2002 .

[44]  T. Shallice,et al.  The origins of utilization behaviour. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  Malcolm A Binns,et al.  Dissociations within the anterior attentional system: effects of task complexity and irrelevant information on reaction time speed and accuracy. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[46]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[47]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Deficits in the Decision-Making Cognition of Chronic Amphetamine Abusers, Opiate Abusers, Patients with Focal Damage to Prefrontal Cortex, and Tryptophan-Depleted Normal Volunteers: Evidence for Monoaminergic Mechanisms , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[48]  D. Broadbent,et al.  The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. , 1982, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[49]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Anomalies in Intertemporal Choice: Evidence and an Interpretation , 1992 .

[50]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  K. Brady,et al.  Impulsivity and rapid discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. , 2003, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[52]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[53]  Reid Hastie,et al.  Phineas gauged: decision-making and the human prefrontal cortex , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Martin P Paulus,et al.  Trend detection via temporal difference model predicts inferior prefrontal cortex activation during acquisition of advantageous action selection , 2004, NeuroImage.

[55]  W. Bickel,et al.  Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. , 2001, Addiction.

[56]  T. Robbins,et al.  Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[57]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Dissociating Valence of Outcome from Behavioral Control in Human Orbital and Ventral Prefrontal Cortices , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  M. Farah,et al.  Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Inhibitory Control is Slowed in Patients with Right Superior Medial Frontal Damage , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.