Use of Landmark Configuration in Pigeons and Humans: II. Generality Across Search Tasks

Pigeons and humans searched for a goal that was hidden in varied locations within a search space. The goal location was fixed relative to an array of identical landmarks. Pigeons searched on the laboratory floor, and humans searched on a table top or an outdoor field. In Experiment 1, the goal was centered in a square array of 4 landmarks. When the spacing between landmarks was increased, humans searched in the middle of the expanded array, whereas pigeons searched in locations that preserved distance and direction to an individual landmark. In Experiment 2, the goal was centered between and a perpendicular distance away from 2 landmarks aligned in the left-fight dimension. When landmark spacing was increased, humans, but not pigeons, shifted their searching away from the landmarks along the perpendicular axis. These results parallel those obtained in touch-screen tasks. Thus, pigeons and humans differ in how they use landmark configuration.

[1]  Thomas S. Collett,et al.  Landmark learning and guidance in insects , 1992 .

[2]  J. Falk Physiology and Behavior. , 1973 .

[3]  K. Cheng,et al.  The determination of direction in landmark-based spatial search in pigeons: A further test of the vector sum model , 1994 .

[4]  David F. Sherry,et al.  Landmark-based spatial memory in birds (Parus atricapillus and Columba livia): The use of edges and distances to represent spatial positions. , 1992 .

[5]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[6]  Marcia L. Spetch,et al.  Pigeons′ Use of Landmarks Presented in Digitized Images , 1994 .

[7]  Marcia L. Spetch,et al.  Control of pigeons' spatial search by graphic landmarks in a touch-screen task. , 1993 .

[8]  M L Spetch,et al.  Overshadowing in landmark learning: touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[10]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[11]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[12]  O. Güntürkün Lateralization of visually controlled behavior in pigeons , 1985, Physiology & Behavior.

[13]  J. Krebs,et al.  Memory for the location of stored food in marsh tits , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[15]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Learning the configuration of a landmark array: I. Touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans. , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[16]  A. Black,et al.  Stimulus control of spatial behavior on the eight-arm maze in rats ☆ ☆☆ , 1980 .

[17]  Marcia L. Spetch,et al.  Landmark use by pigeons in a touch-screen spatial search task , 1992 .

[18]  H. P. Zeigier,et al.  Vision, brain, and behavior in birds. , 1994 .

[19]  K. Cheng The vector sum model of pigeon landmark use. , 1989 .

[20]  Stephen B. Vander Wall,et al.  An experimental analysis of cache recovery in Clark's nutcracker , 1982, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Landmark-Based Spatial Memory in the Pigeon , 1995 .

[22]  Onur Güntürkün,et al.  Visual memory lateralization in pigeons , 1990, Neuropsychologia.