Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later

We examined the neural basis of self-regulation in individuals from a cohort of preschoolers who performed the delay-of-gratification task 4 decades ago. Nearly 60 individuals, now in their mid-forties, were tested on “hot” and “cool” versions of a go/nogo task to assess whether delay of gratification in childhood predicts impulse control abilities and sensitivity to alluring cues (happy faces). Individuals who were less able to delay gratification in preschool and consistently showed low self-control abilities in their twenties and thirties performed more poorly than did high delayers when having to suppress a response to a happy face but not to a neutral or fearful face. This finding suggests that sensitivity to environmental hot cues plays a significant role in individuals’ ability to suppress actions toward such stimuli. A subset of these participants (n = 26) underwent functional imaging for the first time to test for biased recruitment of frontostriatal circuitry when required to suppress responses to alluring cues. Whereas the prefrontal cortex differentiated between nogo and go trials to a greater extent in high delayers, the ventral striatum showed exaggerated recruitment in low delayers. Thus, resistance to temptation as measured originally by the delay-of-gratification task is a relatively stable individual difference that predicts reliable biases in frontostriatal circuitries that integrate motivational and control processes.

[1]  Todd A. Hare,et al.  Frontostriatal Maturation Predicts Cognitive Control Failure to Appetitive Cues in Adolescents , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Nicole L. Wilson,et al.  'Willpower' over the life span: decomposing self-regulation. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[3]  S. Rivera,et al.  Behavioral Assessment of Emotion Discrimination, Emotion Regulation, and Cognitive Control in Childhood, Adolescence, and Adulthood , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[4]  T. Heatherton,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  L. Steinberg,et al.  Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain's reward circuitry. , 2011, Developmental science.

[6]  T. Robbins,et al.  Impulsivity, Compulsivity, and Top-Down Cognitive Control , 2011, Neuron.

[7]  J. Heckman,et al.  A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  David M. Sobel,et al.  Children's beliefs about the fantasy/reality status of hypothesized machines. , 2011, Developmental science.

[9]  L. Somerville,et al.  Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[10]  J. Tanaka,et al.  The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants , 2009, Psychiatry Research.

[11]  S. Buka,et al.  Early manifestations of personality and adult health: a life course perspective. , 2009, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[12]  W. Eaton,et al.  A prospective study of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and coronary heart disease in women. , 2009, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[13]  Colin Camerer,et al.  A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  G. Glover,et al.  Biological Substrates of Emotional Reactivity and Regulation in Adolescence During an Emotional Go-Nogo Task , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  G. Downey,et al.  Rejection Sensitivity and Executive Control: Joint predictors of Borderline Personality features. , 2008, Journal of research in personality.

[16]  A. Sanfey Social Decision-Making : Insights from Game Theory and Neuroscience , 2022 .

[17]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Time Discounting for Primary Rewards , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  G. Glover,et al.  Earlier Development of the Accumbens Relative to Orbitofrontal Cortex Might Underlie Risk-Taking Behavior in Adolescents , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Vivian Zayas,et al.  Predicting Cognitive Control From Preschool to Late Adolescence and Young Adulthood , 2006, Psychological science.

[20]  J. Jonides,et al.  Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[21]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum , 2006, Neuron.

[22]  A. Bechara Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  G. Glover,et al.  The Role of Ventral Frontostriatal Circuitry in Reward-Based Learning in Humans , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Laurence Steinberg,et al.  Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[25]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  G. Glover,et al.  Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[27]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion , 2004, NeuroImage.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Gary H Glover,et al.  Improved combination of spiral‐in/out images for BOLD fMRI , 2004, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[30]  W. Mischel,et al.  Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. , 2004 .

[31]  B. J. Casey,et al.  The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[32]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic approaches toward a model of cognitive control. , 2002, Developmental psychobiology.

[33]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[34]  L. Nystrom,et al.  Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  M. Posner,et al.  Developing mechanisms of self-regulation , 2000, Development and Psychopathology.

[36]  E. Crone,et al.  Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  G. Downey,et al.  Regulating the interpersonal self: strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. , 1999, Psychological review.

[39]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[41]  Temperament and the development of personality. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[42]  J. Cohen,et al.  Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. , 1992, Psychological review.

[43]  Walter Mischel,et al.  Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. , 1990 .

[44]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[45]  W. Mischel,et al.  Cognitive person variables in the delay of gratification of older children at risk. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[47]  W. Mischel,et al.  The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  A. Allport,et al.  Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .

[49]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The cost of visual filtering. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  W. Mischel,et al.  Instrumental ideation in delay of gratification. , 1974, Child development.

[51]  E B Ebbesen,et al.  Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. , 1972, Journal of personality and social psychology.