Independent Distractor Frequency and Age-of-Acquisition Effects in Picture–Word Interference: fMRI Evidence for Post-lexical and Lexical Accounts according to Distractor Type
暂无分享,去创建一个
Katie L. McMahon | Greig I. de Zubicaray | Niels O. Schiller | Michele Miozzo | Kori Johnson | G. Zubicaray | M. Miozzo | K. Mcmahon | Kori Johnson | N. Schiller
[1] Arturo E. Hernandez,et al. Age of acquisition and word frequency: Determinants of object-naming speed and accuracy , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[2] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Semantic interference in a delayed naming task: evidence for the response exclusion hypothesis. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[3] K. Rayner,et al. Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: the frequency-lag hypothesis. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[4] H. E. Brown,et al. Utilizing hemodynamic delay and dispersion to detect fMRI signal change without auditory interference: The behavior interleaved gradients technique , 1999, Magnetic resonance in medicine.
[5] W. Heij,et al. Components of Stroop-like interference in picture naming , 1988, Memory & cognition.
[6] G. Altmann,et al. The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics , 2007 .
[7] G. Humphreys,et al. Semantic interference effects on naming using a postcue procedure: Tapping the links between semantics and phonology with pictures and words. , 1995 .
[8] Kevin G. Munhall,et al. Functional Overlap between Regions Involved in Speech Perception and in Monitoring One's Own Voice during Speech Production , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] Dennis Norris,et al. The Bayesian reader: explaining word recognition as an optimal Bayesian decision process. , 2006, Psychological review.
[10] B. Shaywitz,et al. Paying attention to reading: The neurobiology of reading and dyslexia , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.
[11] John Ashburner,et al. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm , 2007, NeuroImage.
[12] Vincent L. Gracco,et al. On the selection of words and oral motor responses: Evidence of a response-independent fronto-parietal network , 2010, Cortex.
[13] Frank Oppermann,et al. Is there semantic interference in delayed naming? , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[14] Alexander Hammers,et al. Three‐dimensional maximum probability atlas of the human brain, with particular reference to the temporal lobe , 2003, Human brain mapping.
[15] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. Are speech error patterns affected by a monitoring bias? , 2006 .
[16] Herbert Schriefers,et al. Semantic interference in immediate and delayed naming and reading: Attention and task decisions , 2011 .
[17] W. Levelt,et al. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components , 2004, Cognition.
[18] Simon B. Eickhoff,et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.
[19] P. Bonin,et al. Norms and comprehension times for 305 French idiomatic expressions , 2013, Behavior research methods.
[20] Cristina Burani,et al. What causes the effect of age of acquisition in lexical processing? , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[21] Stéphanie Riès,et al. General-Purpose Monitoring during Speech Production , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[22] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] Karl J. Friston,et al. Classical and Bayesian Inference in Neuroimaging: Theory , 2002, NeuroImage.
[24] Ardi Roelofs,et al. From Popper to Lakatos: A case for cumulative computational modeling , 2005 .
[25] K. Mcmahon,et al. Improving EPI imaging quality and sound levels with bandwidth selection , 2004 .
[26] Mart Bles,et al. Phonological processing of ignored distractor pictures, an fMRI investigation , 2008, BMC Neuroscience.
[27] Matthew H. Davis,et al. Mix, a program for pseudorandomization , 2006, Behavior research methods.
[28] Robert A. Johnston,et al. Age of Acquisition, Word Frequency, and Picture–Word Interference , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[29] Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al. The Large-Scale Structure of Semantic Networks: Statistical Analyses and a Model of Semantic Growth , 2001, Cogn. Sci..
[30] Francois-Xavier Alario,et al. Distinct representations of phonemes, syllables, and supra-syllabic sequences in the speech production network , 2009, NeuroImage.
[31] Jean-Francois Mangin,et al. What is the best similarity measure for motion correction in fMRI time series? , 2002, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.
[32] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. Age of acquisition effects in adult lexical processing reflect loss of plasticity in maturing systems: insights from connectionist networks. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] Christopher Barry,et al. Age of Acquisition, Word Frequency, and the Locus of Repetition Priming of Picture Naming , 2001 .
[34] K Ugurbil,et al. Detunable transverse electromagnetic (TEM) volume coil for high‐field NMR , 2002, Magnetic resonance in medicine.
[35] G I de Zubicaray,et al. The semantic interference effect in the picture‐word paradigm: An event‐related fMRI study employing overt responses , 2001, Human brain mapping.
[36] Rainer Goebel,et al. The temporal characteristics of functional activation in Broca's area during overt picture naming , 2009, Cortex.
[37] A. Roelofs,et al. A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.
[38] Michael J Cortese,et al. Age of acquisition predicts naming and lexical-decision performance above and beyond 22 other predictor variables: An analysis of 2,342 words , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[39] Michael J Cortese,et al. Do the effects of subjective frequency and age of acquisition survive better word frequency norms? , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[40] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[41] Katie L. McMahon,et al. Semantic interference in object naming: An fMRI study of the postcue naming paradigm , 2010, NeuroImage.
[42] J. C. Johnston,et al. The role of spatial attention in visual word processing. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] Marc Brysbaert,et al. The effect of age of acquisition: Partly frequency related, partly frequency independent , 2006 .
[44] L. Cohen,et al. The role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in word production , 2006, Brain Research.
[45] Katie L. McMahon,et al. Orthographic/Phonological Facilitation of Naming Responses in the Picture–Word Task: An Event-Related fMRI Study Using Overt Vocal Responding , 2002, NeuroImage.
[46] E. Formisano,et al. Neural correlates of verbal feedback processing: An fMRI study employing overt speech , 2007, Human brain mapping.
[47] A. W. Ellis,et al. Traces of vocabulary acquisition in the brain: Evidence from covert object naming , 2006, NeuroImage.
[48] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[49] Chang H. Lee,et al. PHONOLOGICAL RECODING OF MIXED-CASE WORDS IN THE PRIMING TASK , 2002 .
[50] Albert Costa,et al. The effect of word-frequency on lexical selection in speech production: Evidence from semantic homogeneous naming contexts , 2006 .
[51] C. Price. The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009 , 2010, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[52] Gary S. Dell,et al. Speech Errors and Language Production: Neuropsychological and Connectionist Perspectives. , 2003 .
[53] Katie McMahon,et al. Top‐down influences on lexical selection during spoken word production: A 4T fMRI investigation of refractory effects in picture naming , 2006, Human brain mapping.
[54] Jonathan C Catling,et al. The varying effects of age of acquisition , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[55] John J. L. Morton,et al. Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .
[56] K I Forster,et al. The Pros and Cons of Masked Priming , 1998, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[57] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Age of acquisition effects in picture naming: evidence for a lexical-semantic competition hypothesis , 2005, Cognition.
[58] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. Age of acquisition effects depend on the mapping between representations and the frequency of occurrence: Empirical and computational evidence , 2006 .
[59] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Age of Acquisition Effects in Word Reading and Other Tasks , 2002 .
[60] R. Hartsuiker,et al. The distractor frequency effect in picture-word interference: Evidence for response exclusion. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[61] Vincent L. Gracco,et al. Imaging speech production using fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.
[62] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Multiple perspectives on word production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[63] P. Starreveld,et al. Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. , 1996 .
[64] Sheila E. Blumstein,et al. Neural Systems underlying Lexical Competition: An Eye Tracking and fMRI Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[65] Kevin Dent,et al. Age of acquisition and word frequency effects in picture naming: a dual-task investigation. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[66] Herbert Schriefers,et al. Selective attention and distractor frequency in naming performance: comment on Dhooge and Hartsuiker (2010). , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[67] Ardi Roelofs,et al. Attention to Spoken Word Planning: Chronometric and Neuroimaging Evidence , 2008, Lang. Linguistics Compass.
[68] R. Bowtell,et al. The effect of scanner sound in visual, motor, and auditory functional MRI , 1999, Magnetic resonance in medicine.
[69] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[70] Katrin Amunts,et al. A systems perspective on the effective connectivity of overt speech production , 2009, NeuroImage.
[71] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[72] Karl J. Friston,et al. Classical and Bayesian Inference in Neuroimaging: Applications , 2002, NeuroImage.
[73] A. Caramazza,et al. When more is less: a counterintuitive effect of distractor frequency in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[74] Daniel J. Acheson,et al. A Common Neural Substrate for Language Production and Verbal Working Memory , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[75] Katrin Amunts,et al. The determiner congruency effect in language production investigated with functional MRI , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[76] Trevor A. Harley,et al. Phonological activation of semantic competitors during lexical access in speech production , 1993 .
[77] D. Bub,et al. Interference from related items in object identification. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[78] R. Golinkoff,et al. Automatic semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. , 1975 .
[79] Rajesh Kumar,et al. A method for removal of global effects from fMRI time series , 2004, NeuroImage.
[80] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[81] Piers L. Cornelissen,et al. Interactions between visual and semantic processing during object recognition revealed by modulatory effects of age of acquisition , 2014, NeuroImage.