Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task

Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulata) were subjected to a place finding task in a rectangular room perfectly homogeneous and without distinctive featural information. Results of Experiment 1 show that monkeys rely on the large-scale geometry of the room to retrieve a food reward. Experiments 2 and 3 indicate that subjects use also nongeometric information (colored wall) to reorient. Data of Experiments 4 and 5 suggest that monkeys do not use small angular cues but that they are sensitive to the size of the cues (Experiments 6, 7, and 8). Our findings strengthen the idea that a mechanism based on the geometry of the environment is at work in several mammalian species. In addition, the present data offer new perspectives on spatial cognition in animals that are phylogenetically close to humans. Specifically, the joint use of both geometric and landmark-based cues by rhesus monkeys tends to demonstrate that spatial processing became more flexible with evolution.

[1]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Cognitive Processes and Spatial Orientation in Animal and Man , 1987 .

[2]  W. K. Honig,et al.  Cognitive Processes in Animal Behavior , 1979 .

[3]  Debbie M. Kelly,et al.  Pigeons' (Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. , 1998 .

[4]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Mechanisms of reorientation and object localization by children: A comparison with rats , 1999 .

[5]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[6]  Adèle,et al.  The Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cognitive Functions. A conference. May 20-24, 1989, Philadelphia, Pa. , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[7]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[8]  P. Menyuk Cognition and Language. , 1976 .

[9]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape , 1988 .

[10]  E. Menzel Chimpanzee Spatial Memory Organization , 1973, Science.

[11]  J. Vauclair,et al.  Reaction To Novel Objects in a Troop of Guinea Baboons: Approach and Manipulation , 1986 .

[12]  A. Black,et al.  Stimulus control of spatial behavior on the eight-arm maze in rats ☆ ☆☆ , 1980 .

[13]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  Terry Kit-fong Au,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive development , 1996 .

[15]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  A Study of Spatial Parameters Encoded During Exploration in Hamsters , 1987 .

[16]  R. Cook,et al.  Landmark geometry and identity controls spatial navigation in rats , 1997 .

[17]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  LANDMARK USE BY NAVIGATING RATS (RATTUS NORVEGICUS) : CONTRASTING GEOMETRIC AND FEATURAL INFORMATION , 1998 .

[18]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[19]  J. Taube,et al.  Effects of repeated disorientation on the acquisition of spatial tasks in rats: dissociation between the appetitive radial arm maze and aversive water maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[20]  R Biegler,et al.  Landmark stability: studies exploring whether the perceived stability of the environment influences spatial representation. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[21]  L. Acredolo,et al.  Behavioral Approaches to Spatial Orientation in Infancy , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[22]  R. Biegler,et al.  Landmark stability is a prerequisite for spatial but not discrimination learning , 1993, Nature.

[23]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[24]  L. Hermer-Vazquez,et al.  Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks , 2001, Cognition.

[25]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). , 1990, Journal of comparative psychology.

[26]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[27]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[28]  K. Cheng Rats Use the Geometry of Surfaces for Navigation , 1987 .

[29]  E. Spelke,et al.  Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.

[30]  K. Milton Distribution Patterns of Tropical Plant Foods as an Evolutionary Stimulus to Primate Mental Development , 1981 .

[31]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampal Place Fields, the Internal Compass, and the Learning of Landmark Stability, , 1994 .

[32]  Alan C. Kamil,et al.  The seed-storing corvid Clark's nutcracker learns geometric relationships among landmarks , 1997, Nature.

[33]  S. Gouteux,et al.  Reaction to spatial novelty and exploratory strategies in baboons , 1999 .

[34]  O. L. Tinklepaugh The multiple delayed reaction with chimpanzees and monkeys. , 1932 .

[35]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .