Collinear contextual suppression

The context of a target can modulate behavioral as well as neural responses to that target. For example, target processing can be suppressed by iso-oriented surrounds whereas it can be facilitated by collinear contextual elements. Here, we present experiments in which collinear elements exert strong suppression whereas iso-oriented contextual surrounds yield no contextual modulation--contrary to most studies in this field. We suggest that contextual suppression depends strongly on the spatial arrangement of the context pointing to the influence of Gestalt factors in contextual modulation.

[1]  Christof Koch,et al.  Spatial aspects of object formation revealed by a new illusion, shine-through , 2001, Vision Research.

[2]  Z Li,et al.  Pre-attentive segmentation in the primary visual cortex. , 1998, Spatial vision.

[3]  C. Koch,et al.  Flanker effects in peripheral contrast discrimination—psychophysics and modeling , 2001, Vision Research.

[4]  Wu Li,et al.  Global contour saliency and local colinear interactions. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  A. Giersch A new pharmacological tool to investigate integration processes , 1999 .

[6]  B. Breitmeyer Disinhibition in metacontrast masking of vernier acuity targets: Sustained channels inhibit transient channels , 1978, Vision Research.

[7]  J. Allman,et al.  Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[9]  Michael H Herzog,et al.  Timing of contextual modulation in the shine-through effect , 2003, Vision Research.

[10]  C. Wehrhahn,et al.  Detection facilitation by collinear stimuli in humans: Dependence on strength and sign of contrast , 1998, Vision Research.

[11]  M. Morgan,et al.  Facilitation from collinear flanks is cancelled by non-collinear flanks , 2000, Vision Research.

[12]  R. Shapley,et al.  Contextual influences on orientation discrimination: binding local and global cues , 2001, Vision Research.

[13]  M. Bach,et al.  The Freiburg Visual Acuity test--automatic measurement of visual acuity. , 1996, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[14]  U. Polat,et al.  Collinear stimuli regulate visual responses depending on cell's contrast threshold , 1998, Nature.

[15]  Michael H. Herzog,et al.  Local Interactions in Neural Networks Explain Global Effects in Gestalt Processing and Masking , 2003, Neural Computation.

[16]  J. Bullier,et al.  Response modulations by static texture surround in area V1 of the macaque monkey do not depend on feedback connections from V2. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.

[18]  Norbert Krüger,et al.  Collinearity and Parallelism are Statistically Significant Second-Order Relations of Complex Cell Responses , 1998, Neural Processing Letters.

[19]  G Westheimer,et al.  Integration of foveal orientation signals: distinct local and long-range spatial domains. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  H. Jones,et al.  Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities , 1995, Nature.

[21]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[22]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[23]  C. Gilbert,et al.  On a common circle: natural scenes and Gestalt rules. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys , 1995, Neuron.

[25]  G Westheimer,et al.  Patterns That Impair Discrimination of Line Orientation in Human Vision , 1996, Perception.

[26]  Christof Koch,et al.  Switching binding states , 2001 .

[27]  Victor A. F. Lamme The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  Michael H. Herzog,et al.  Effects of grouping in contextual modulation , 2002, Nature.

[29]  Lynn A Olzak,et al.  Multiple gain control processes in contrast–contrast phenomena , 1999, Vision Research.

[30]  D. Heeger,et al.  Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppression and enhancement , 2001, Vision Research.

[31]  J A Solomon,et al.  Texture interactions determine perceived contrast , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Z Li,et al.  Visual segmentation by contextual influences via intra-cortical interactions in the primary visual cortex. , 1999, Network.

[33]  Birgitta Dresp Bright lines and edges facilitate the detection of small light targets. , 1993, Spatial vision.

[34]  C Wehrhahn,et al.  Contextual influence on orientation discrimination of humans and responses of neurons in V1 of alert monkeys. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Topography of contextual modulations mediated by short-range interactions in primary visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[36]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Contextual Modulation in Primary Visual Cortex , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Rolf P. Würtz,et al.  Learning the Gestalt Rule of Collinearity from Object Motion , 2003, Neural Computation.

[38]  W Singer,et al.  The Perceptual Grouping Criterion of Colinearity is Reflected by Anisotropies of Connections in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[39]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  U Polat,et al.  Collinear effects on 3-Gabor alignment as a function of spacing, orientation and detectability. , 2001, Spatial vision.

[41]  A. Watson,et al.  Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks , 1999, Vision Research.

[42]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Response modulation by texture surround in primate area V1: Correlates of “popout” under anesthesia , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[43]  Yoshimichi Ejima,et al.  Apparent contrast of a sinusoidal grating in the simultaneous presence of peripheral gratings , 1985, Vision Research.

[44]  C Koch,et al.  Seeing properties of an invisible object: Feature inheritance and shine-through , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  N Weisstein,et al.  Line segments are perceived better in a coherent context than alone: An object-line effect in visual perception , 1978, Memory & cognition.

[46]  Mark W. Cannon,et al.  Spatial interactions in apparent contrast: Inhibitory effects among grating patterns of different spatial frequencies, spatial positions and orientations , 1991, Vision Research.

[47]  S. J. Harrison,et al.  Within‐texture alignment improves human texture segmentation , 2002 .

[48]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[49]  D. Heeger,et al.  Center-surround interactions in foveal and peripheral vision , 2000, Vision Research.

[50]  M. M. Taylor,et al.  PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions , 1967 .

[51]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Spatial distribution of contextual interactions in primary visual cortex and in visual perception. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Fredric M. Wolf,et al.  The prevalence of colinear contours in the real world , 2001, Neurocomputing.

[53]  N Weisstein,et al.  Visual Detection of Line Segments: An Object-Superiority Effect , 1974, Science.

[54]  D. Sagi,et al.  Isolating Excitatory and Inhibitory Nonlinear Spatial Interactions Involved in Contrast Detection * * Part of this paper was presented at the 17th ECVP conference, Eindhoven, The Netherlands (September 1994). , 1996, Vision Research.

[55]  M. Sur,et al.  Subthreshold facilitation and suppression in primary visual cortex revealed by intrinsic signal imaging. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Characteristics of surround inhibition in cat area 17 , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  A. Giersch The effects of lorazepam on visual integration processes: How useful for neuroscientists? , 2001 .

[58]  G Westheimer,et al.  Spatial and temporal integration of signals in foveal line orientation. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  Tzvetomir Tzvetanov,et al.  Short- and long-range effects in line contrast integration , 2002, Vision Research.

[60]  Zoltán Vidnyánszky,et al.  Contextual modulation of orientation discrimination is independent of stimulus processing time , 2001, Vision Research.