Cognitive mapping in mental time travel and mental space navigation

The ability to imagine ourselves in the past, in the future or in different spatial locations suggests that the brain can generate cognitive maps that are independent of the experiential self in the here and now. Using three experiments, we asked to which extent Mental Time Travel (MTT; imagining the self in time) and Mental Space Navigation (MSN; imagining the self in space) shared similar cognitive operations. For this, participants judged the ordinality of real historical events in time and in space with respect to different mental perspectives: for instance, participants mentally projected themselves in Paris in nine years, and judged whether an event occurred before or after, or, east or west, of where they mentally stood. In all three experiments, symbolic distance effects in time and space dimensions were quantified using Reaction Times (RT) and Error Rates (ER). When self-projected, participants were slower and were less accurate (absolute distance effects); participants were also faster and more accurate when the spatial and temporal distances were further away from their mental viewpoint (relative distance effects). These effects show that MTT and MSN require egocentric mapping and that self-projection requires map transformations. Additionally, participants' performance was affected when self-projection was made in one dimension but judgements in another, revealing a competition between temporal and spatial mapping (Experiment 2 & 3). Altogether, our findings suggest that MTT and MSN are separately mapped although they require comparable allo- to ego-centric map conversion.

[1]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Traversing psychological distance , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  S. Monsell Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  R. Shepard,et al.  Perceptual illusion of rotation of three-dimensional objects. , 1976, Science.

[4]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[5]  E. Tulving Episodic and semantic memory: Where should we go from here? , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[7]  C. R. Gallistel,et al.  Chapter 1 – Mental Magnitudes , 2011 .

[8]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[9]  E. Tulving,et al.  Episodic and semantic memory , 1972 .

[10]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Comprar Space, Time And Number In The Brain, Searching For The Foundations Of Mathematical Thought | S. Dehaene | 9780123859488 | Academic Press , 2011 .

[11]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Asohan Amarasingham,et al.  Hippocampus Internally Generated Cell Assembly Sequences in the Rat , 2011 .

[13]  Wesley A. Mah,et al.  Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitude , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  M. Zorzi,et al.  When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  Carolyn Parkinson,et al.  A Common Cortical Metric for Spatial, Temporal, and Social Distance , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Asohan Amarasingham,et al.  Internally Generated Cell Assembly Sequences in the Rat Hippocampus , 2008, Science.

[18]  R. D. Easton,et al.  Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[20]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[22]  R. Moyer,et al.  Mental comparison and the symbolic distance effect , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Donna Rose Addis,et al.  Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  John M. Parkman,et al.  Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. , 1971, Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[25]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  The mental time line: An analogue of the mental number line in the mapping of life events , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[26]  D C Rubin,et al.  The distribution of autobiographical memories across the lifespan , 1997, Memory & cognition.

[27]  T. McNamara,et al.  Intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[29]  H. Eichenbaum Memory on time , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[31]  John A. King,et al.  Memory for events and their spatial context: models and experiments. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[32]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[34]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Subjective mental time: the functional architecture of projecting the self to past and future , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[35]  B. Mellers,et al.  Measurement and the mental map , 1978 .

[36]  S. Dehaene Subtracting Pigeons: Logarithmic or Linear? , 2001, Psychological science.

[37]  H. H. Clark The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .

[38]  Edvard I. Moser,et al.  Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex , 2015, Nature.

[39]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Memory and the Computational Brain , 2009 .

[40]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[41]  Michaël A. Stevens,et al.  A model of exact small-number representation , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  Emilio Kropff,et al.  Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[43]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network , 2010, Nature.

[44]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[45]  B. Poucet Spatial cognitive maps in animals: new hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms. , 1993, Psychological review.

[46]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[47]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[49]  S M Kosslyn,et al.  Visual images preserve metric spatial information: evidence from studies of image scanning. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  M. Corballis,et al.  The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? , 2007, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[51]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Self in Time: Imagined Self-Location Influences Neural Activity Related to Mental Time Travel , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Distortions in cognitive maps , 1992 .

[53]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Space, time and number in the brain : searching for the foundations of mathematical thought , 2011 .

[54]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.