Group intervention changes brain activity in bilingual language-impaired children.

This investigation assessed the effectiveness of a phonological intervention program on the brain functioning of bilingual Finnish 6- to 7-year-old preschool children diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI). The intervention program was implemented by preschool teachers to small groups of children including children with SLI. A matched group of other bilingual children with SLI received a physical exercise program and served as a control group. Auditory evoked magnetic fields were measured before and after the intervention with an oddball paradigm. The brain activity recordings were followed by a behavioral discrimination test. Our results show that, in children with SLI, the positive intervention effect is reflected in plastic changes in the brain activity of the left and right auditory cortices.

[1]  Nina Kraus,et al.  Neural plasticity following auditory training in children with learning problems , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[2]  H. Neville,et al.  The Neurobiology of Sensory and Language Processing in Language-Impaired Children , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Ingvar Lundberg,et al.  Effects of an Extensive Program for Stimulating Phonological Awareness in Preschool Children. , 1988 .

[4]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. , 1993, Neuroreport.

[5]  Valerie L. Shafer,et al.  Neurophysiological Indexes of Speech Processing Deficits in Children with Specific Language Impairment , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .

[7]  K. Juottonen,et al.  Abnormal Frequency Mismatch Negativity in Mentally Retarded Children and in Children With Developmental Dysphasia , 1998, Journal of child neurology.

[8]  Carolyn Kessler,et al.  Language Minority Children's Linguistic and Cognitive Creativity. , 1987 .

[9]  Improved Language Skills by Children with Low Reading Performance who used Fast ForWord Language 1 MAPS for Learning : Product Report 3 ( 1 ) 1 : 13 , 2005 .

[10]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Näätänen,et al.  The mismatch negativity in evaluating central auditory dysfunction in dyslexia , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses , 1997, Nature.

[13]  R. Näätänen Attention and brain function , 1992 .

[14]  Linda J. Lombardino,et al.  Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children , 1997, Nature.

[15]  J. Tomblin,et al.  Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[16]  L. Verhoeven,et al.  Classification of developmental language disorders. , 2004 .

[17]  M. Stuart,et al.  Phonological Awareness in Monolingual and Bilingual English and Greek Five-year-olds. , 2003 .

[18]  D. V. M. Bishop,et al.  Which People with Specific Language Impairment have Auditory Processing Deficits? , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[19]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Responses of the primary auditory cortex to pitch changes in a sequence of tone pips: Neuromagnetic recordings in man , 1984, Neuroscience Letters.

[20]  H. Rubin,et al.  Linguistic awareness skills in grade one children in a French immersion setting , 1989 .

[21]  P. Kuhl Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  H. Persson,et al.  Topography of auditory evoked cortical potentials in children with severe language impairment: the N1 component. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[23]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Temporal Processing Deficits of Language-Learning Impaired Children Ameliorated by Training , 1996, Science.

[24]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Ambiguous benefits: the effect of bilingualism on reversing ambiguous figures. , 2005, Developmental science.

[25]  Ellen Bialystok,et al.  The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving , 1998, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[26]  Massimiliano Valeriani,et al.  Attentional load of the primary task influences the frontal but not the temporal generators of mismatch negativity. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[27]  Minna Huotilainen,et al.  Newborns discriminate novel from harmonic sounds: A study using magnetoencephalography , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. , 2005, Developmental science.

[29]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Magnetoencephalography-theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain , 1993 .

[30]  D. Bishop Specific Language Impairment: Diagnostic Dilemmas , 2003 .

[31]  Larry E. Roberts,et al.  Plastic changes in the auditory cortex induced by intensive frequency discrimination training , 2000, Neuroreport.

[32]  Krista L. Johnson,et al.  Learning impaired children exhibit timing deficits and training-related improvements in auditory cortical responses to speech in noise , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Early Speech Perception and Later Language Development: Implications for the "Critical Period" , 2005 .

[34]  P. Tallal,et al.  Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[35]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Test–retest stability of the magnetic mismatch response (MMNm) , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[36]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Language Comprehension in Language-Learning Impaired Children Improved with Acoustically Modified Speech , 1996, Science.

[37]  R Näätänen,et al.  The mismatch negativity as an index of temporal processing in audition , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[38]  M. Dorman,et al.  Neurophysiologic correlates of cross-language phonetic perception. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[39]  C. Elberling,et al.  Auditory magnetic fields from the human cerebral cortex: Location and strength of an equivalent current dipole , 1982, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[40]  P. Korpilahti Auditory discrimination and memory functions in SLI children: A comprehensive study with neurophysiological and behavioural methods , 1995 .

[41]  Martha Crago,et al.  French-English bilingual children with SLI: how do they compare with their monolingual peers? , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[42]  G. Conti-Ramsden,et al.  Bilingualism and specific language impairment in children attending language units. , 1997, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.

[43]  Elvira Brattico,et al.  Effects of brief discrimination-training on the auditory N1 wave , 2003, Neuroreport.

[44]  W. Espeland,et al.  Low intensity phonological awareness training in a preschool classroom for children with communication impairments. , 2005, Journal of communication disorders.

[45]  P. Alku,et al.  A method for generating natural-sounding speech stimuli for cognitive brain research , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[46]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Auditory magnetic responses of healthy newborns , 2003, Neuroreport.

[47]  Paula Tallal,et al.  Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills , 1985, Brain and Language.

[48]  A. Friederici,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for delayed mismatch response in infants at-risk for specific language impairment. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[49]  A. V. Kleeck,et al.  A Study of Classroom-Based Phonological Awareness Training for Preschoolers With Speech and/or Language Disorders , 1998 .

[50]  H. Renvall,et al.  Diminished auditory mismatch fields in dyslexic adults , 2003, Annals of neurology.

[51]  Lynette Bradley,et al.  Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling , 1985 .

[52]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Speech-sound discrimination in neonates as measured with MEG , 2004, Neuroreport.

[53]  R. Hari,et al.  Responses of the human auditory cortex to vowel onset after fricative consonants , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Dc Washington Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. , 1994 .

[55]  P. Alku,et al.  Magnetic fields evoked by speech sounds in preschool children , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[56]  M. Bruck,et al.  Phonological awareness in young second language learners , 1995, Journal of Child Language.

[57]  C. Pallier,et al.  A limit on behavioral plasticity in speech perception , 1997, Cognition.

[58]  Ellen Bialystok,et al.  Cardinal Limits: Evidence from Language Awareness and Bilingualism for Developing Concepts of Number. , 1997 .

[59]  J. Eggermont,et al.  Maturation of human central auditory system activity: separating auditory evoked potentials by dipole source modeling , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[60]  Jakob Cromdal,et al.  Childhood bilingualism and metalinguistic skills: Analysis and control in young Swedish–English bilinguals , 1999, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[61]  P. Tallal,et al.  Defects of Non-Verbal Auditory Perception in Children with Developmental Aphasia , 1973, Nature.

[62]  C. Escera,et al.  The accuracy of sound duration representation in the human brain determines the accuracy of behavioural perception , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[63]  P. Tallal,et al.  Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[64]  K. Eklund,et al.  Brain Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) Measured at Birth Predict Later Language Development in Children with and Without Familial Risk for Dyslexia , 2005, Cortex.

[65]  I. Rosén,et al.  Auditory event-related brain potentials in children with specific language impairment. , 2002, European journal of paediatric neurology : EJPN : official journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society.

[66]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: a longitudinal study. , 2004, Child development.

[67]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[68]  T. Carrell,et al.  Auditory Neurophysiologic Responses and Discrimination Deficits in Children with Learning Problems , 1996, Science.

[69]  K. Juottonen,et al.  Attenuated Auditory Event-Related Potential (Mismatch Negativity) in Children with Developmental Dysphasia , 1997, Neuropediatrics.

[70]  B. Gullberg,et al.  Language impairment in Swedish bilingual children: a comparison between bilingual and monolingual children in Malmö , 2002, Acta paediatrica.

[71]  Stuart Rosen,et al.  Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? , 2003, J. Phonetics.

[72]  E. Bialystok Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. , 1988 .

[73]  W. Roberts,et al.  Prominence of M50 auditory evoked response over M100 in childhood and autism , 2004, Neuroreport.

[74]  R. Näätänen The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[75]  P. Korpilahti,et al.  Auditory ERP components and mismatch negativity in dysphasic children. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[76]  Murray J. Munro,et al.  Effects of age of second-language learning on the production of English consonants , 1995, Speech Commun..

[77]  L. Verhoeven,et al.  Classification of developmental language disorders. Theoretical Issues and Clinical Implications , 2004 .

[78]  Toshiaki Imada,et al.  Effects of language experience: Neural commitment to language-specific auditory patterns , 2005, NeuroImage.

[79]  C. Leonard,et al.  The Evolution of Neuroimaging Research and Developmental Language Disorders , 2001 .

[80]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[81]  H. Renvall,et al.  Auditory Cortical Responses to Speech-Like Stimuli in Dyslexic Adults , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[82]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Plastic neural changes and reading improvement caused by audiovisual training in reading-impaired children , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[83]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Attentive novelty detection in humans is governed by pre-attentive sensory memory , 1994, Nature.

[84]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Sensory modulation of auditory stimuli in children with Autism And Receptive Developmental Language Disorder: Event-related brain potential evidence , 1995, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[85]  A. Crutchley Bilingual children in language units: does having 'well-informed' parents make a difference? , 2000, International journal of language and communication disorders.

[86]  Z. Gou,et al.  The infant as a prelinguistic model for language learning impairments: Predicting from event-related potentials to behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[87]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[88]  R. Uwer,et al.  Automatic processing of tones and speech stimuli in children with specific language impairment. , 2002, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[89]  Mara L. Morr,et al.  Deviant neurophysiological asymmetry in children with language impairment , 2000, Neuroreport.

[90]  Janet F. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Developmental change does not involve loss , 1994 .

[91]  S. Taulu,et al.  Applications of the signal space separation method , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Signal Processing.

[92]  Paavo Alku,et al.  Mismatch negativity shows that 3–6-year-old children can learn to discriminate non-native speech sounds within two months , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[93]  Dorothy V. M. Bishop,et al.  Individual Differences in Auditory Processing in Specific Language Impairment: A Follow-Up Study using Event-Related Potentials and Behavioural Thresholds , 2005, Cortex.

[94]  M. Karjalainen,et al.  Short-term memory functions of the human fetus recorded with magnetoencephalography , 2005, Neuroreport.