The evolution of cognition - a hypothesis

Behavior may be controlled by reactive systems. In a reactive system the motor output is exclusively driven by actual sensory input. An alternative solution to control behavior is given by “cognitive” systems capable of planning ahead. To this end the system has to be equipped with some kind of internal world model. A sensible basis of an internal world model might be a model of the system’s own body. I show that a reactive system with the ability to control a body of complex geometry requires only a slight reorganization to form a cognitive system. This implies that the assumption that the evolution of cognitive properties requires the introduction of new, additional modules, namely internal world models, is not justified. Rather, these modules may already have existed before the system obtained cognitive properties. Furthermore, I discuss whether the occurrence of such world models may lead to systems having internal perspective. © 2002 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved.

[1]  J. Decety,et al.  Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[3]  Jun Tani,et al.  An Interpretation of the "Self" From the Dynamical Systems Perspective: A Constructivist Approach , 1998 .

[4]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Foundations of Cognitive Science , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Thomas Metzinger,et al.  The Subjectivity of Subjective Experience: A Representationalist Analysis of the First-Person Perspective. , 2000 .

[6]  J. Kevin O'Regan,et al.  The origin of 'feel' , 2002 .

[7]  A. Damasio The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness , 1999 .

[8]  Holk Cruse,et al.  MMC- a recurrent neural network which can be used as manipulable body model , 1998 .

[9]  Holk Cruse,et al.  Prerational Intelligence: Interdisciplinary perspectives on the behavior of natural and artificial systems , 2000 .

[10]  Helge J. Ritter,et al.  Rapid learning with parametrized self-organizing maps , 1996, Neurocomputing.

[11]  Thomas Kindermann,et al.  Walknet--a biologically inspired network to control six-legged walking , 1998, Neural Networks.

[12]  Domenico Parisi,et al.  Learning in the Active Mode , 1995, ECAL.

[13]  Richard W. Byrne,et al.  Evolution of primate cognition , 2000, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[15]  R. Beer,et al.  Biorobotic approaches to the study of motor systems , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  J. Fuster Memory in the cerebral cortex , 1994 .

[17]  Holk Cruse,et al.  Two kinds of body representation are used to control hand movements following tactile stimulation , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  M. Erb,et al.  Activation of Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas during Executed and Imagined Hand Movements: An fMRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Aaron Sloman,et al.  7. Architectural Requirements for Human-Like Agents Both Natural and Artificial: What sorts of machines can love? , 2000 .

[20]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  G. Miller,et al.  Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.

[23]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Development of young children's understanding that the recent past is causally bound to the present. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[24]  B. Libet,et al.  Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious sensory experience: a functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[26]  E. Rolls High-level vision: Object recognition and visual cognition, Shimon Ullman. MIT Press, Bradford (1996), ISBN 0 262 21013 4 , 1997 .

[27]  Stefano Nolfi,et al.  Learning to perceive the world as articulated: an approach for hierarchical learning in sensory-motor systems , 1998, Neural Networks.

[28]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Error correction and the basal ganglia Response to Lawrence (2000) , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Paul Ekman,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: New Findings, New Questions , 1992 .

[30]  O. Neumann,et al.  Manual and Verbal Responses to Completely Masked (Unreportable) Stimuli: Exploring Some Conditions for the Metacontrast Dissociation , 1998, Perception.

[31]  Nils J. Nilsson,et al.  Artificial Intelligence: A New Synthesis , 1997 .

[32]  J. Lackner Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  Horst-Michael Groß,et al.  Generative character of perception: a neural architecture for sensorimotor anticipation , 1999, Neural Networks.

[34]  Petr Sosík,et al.  Advances in Artificial Life , 2001, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

[35]  Holk Cruse,et al.  A holistic model for an internal representation to control the movement of a manipulator with redundant degrees of freedom , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[36]  H. Cruse,et al.  A network model for the control of the movement of a redundant manipulator , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[37]  Holk Cruse The explanatory power and limits of simulation models in the neurosciences , 2001 .

[38]  M. Jeannerod The cognitive neuroscience of action , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  Pattie Maes,et al.  A bottom-up mechanism for behavior selection in an artificial creature , 1991 .

[40]  H. Cruse,et al.  MMC – a new numerical approach to the kinematics of complex manipulators , 2002 .

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Perceptual Structures and Distributed Motor Control , 1981 .

[43]  Holk Cruse Feeling our body - the basis of cognition? , 1999 .

[44]  Rolf Pfeifer,et al.  Cognition - perspectives from autonomous agents , 1995, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[45]  Rodney A. Brooks,et al.  Intelligence Without Reason , 1991, IJCAI.

[46]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  Rj Dolan,et al.  Phantoms in the brain: Probing the mysteries of the human mind , 1998 .

[48]  R. Melzack Pain and the neuromatrix in the brain. , 2001, Journal of dental education.

[49]  R A Brooks,et al.  New Approaches to Robotics , 1991, Science.

[50]  D. McFarland,et al.  Intelligent behavior in animals and robots , 1993 .

[51]  L. Brakel A Universe of Consciousness: How Matter Becomes Imagination , 2001 .

[52]  de Ritske Jong,et al.  Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[53]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. , 1989, Science.

[54]  David McFarland,et al.  Philosophical Perspectives on Representation, Goals, and Cognition , 2000 .

[55]  H. Maturana,et al.  The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding , 2007 .

[56]  B. Williams Decisions, decisions , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Consciousness: mapping the theoretical landscape , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  M. Jeannerod TO ACT OR NOT TO ACT : PERSPECTIVES ON THE REPRESENTATION OF ACTIONS , 1999 .

[59]  Holk Cruse Modellvorstellungen zu Bewußtseinsvorgängen , 1979 .

[60]  Pietro G. Morasso,et al.  Internal models in the control of posture , 1999, Neural Networks.

[61]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[62]  Masao Ito Movement and thought: identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[63]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Touching the phantom limb , 1995, Nature.

[64]  D. Coder,et al.  How Brains Think , 1973, Dialogue.

[65]  J. J. Hopfield,et al.  “Neural” computation of decisions in optimization problems , 1985, Biological Cybernetics.

[66]  B. Pillon,et al.  Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[67]  J. Pollack The Induction of Dynamical Recognizers , 1996, Machine Learning.

[68]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  H. Sakata,et al.  The TINS Lecture The parietal association cortex in depth perception and visual control of hand action , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[70]  H. Cruse,et al.  Control of Three- and Four-Joint Arm Movement: Strategies for a Manipulator With Redundant Degrees of Freedom. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[71]  Rodney A. Brooks,et al.  From earwigs to humans , 1997, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[72]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[73]  H. Evans The Study of Instinct , 1952 .