Models and mosaics: Investigating cross-cultural differences in risk perception and risk preference

In this article, we describe a multistudy project designed to explain observed cross-national differences in risk taking between respondents from the People’s Republic of China and the United States. Using this example, we develop the following recommendations for cross-cultural investigations. First, like all psychological research, cross-cultural studies should be model based. Investigators should commit themselves to a model of the behavior under study that explicitly specifies possible causal constructs or variables hypothesized to influence the behavior, as well as the relationship between those variables, and allows for individual, group, or cultural differences in the value of these variables or in the relationship between them. This moves the focus from a simple demonstration of cross-national differences toward a prediction of the behavior, including its cross-national variation. Ideally, the causal construct hypothesized and shown to differ between cultures should be demonstrated to serve as a moderator or a mediator between culture and observed behavioral differences. Second, investigators should look for converging evidence for hypothesized cultural effects on behavior by looking at multiple dependent variables and using multiple methodological approaches. Thus, the data collection that will allow for the establishment of conclusive causal connections between a cultural variable and some target behavior can be compared with the creation of a mosaic.

[1]  Rakesh K. Sarin,et al.  Risk-value models , 1993 .

[2]  M. Cole Cultural psychology: a once and future discipline? , 1996, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[3]  J. Brockner,et al.  The moderating roles of self-esteem and self-construal in reaction to a threat to the self: evidence from the People's Republic of China and the United States. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Pierre R. Dasen,et al.  Human behavior in global perspective , 1999 .

[5]  Hermann Jahnke,et al.  Remarks on: "Some Extensions of the Discrete Lotsizing and Scheduling Problem" , 1997 .

[6]  J. Yates,et al.  General knowledge overconfidence : Cross-national variations, response style, and reality , 1997 .

[7]  S. Kitayama Culture and self: Implications for internationalizing psychology. , 1996 .

[8]  Kenneth R. MacCrimmon,et al.  Characteristics of risk taking executives , 1990 .

[9]  G. Hofstede,et al.  Culture′s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values , 1980 .

[10]  F. Gaenslen Culture and Decision Making in China, Japan, Russia, and the United States , 1986, World Politics.

[11]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Cross-National Differences in Risk Preference and Lay Predictions , 1999 .

[12]  Joni Hersch,et al.  SMOKING, SEAT BELTS, AND OTHER RISKY CONSUMER DECISIONS: DIFFERENCES BY GENDER AND RACE , 1996 .

[13]  J. Frank Yates,et al.  Judgment and Decision Making , 1990 .

[14]  I. Erev,et al.  Simultaneous Over- and Underconfidence: The Role of Error in Judgment Processes. , 1994 .

[15]  H. Markus,et al.  Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. , 1991 .

[16]  R. Pollack Pictures, maybe; illusions, no , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  Joel Brockner,et al.  Toward an explanation of cultural differences in in-group favoritism: The role of individual versus collective primacy. , 1998 .

[18]  J. Bruner Acts of meaning , 1990 .

[19]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Cross-Cultural Differences in Risk Perception,But Cross-Cultural Similarities in Attitudes Towards Perceived Risk , 1998 .

[20]  W. Edwards,et al.  Decision Analysis and Behavioral Research , 1986 .

[21]  D. Campbell,et al.  The influence of culture on visual perception , 1967 .

[22]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN DECISION PROCESSES, Vol. 75 Issue 02 , 1998 .

[23]  R. Brislin Back-Translation for Cross-Cultural Research , 1970 .

[24]  E. Weber,et al.  Cross‐Cultural Differences in Risk Perception: A Model‐Based Approach , 1997 .

[25]  David L. Ronis,et al.  Probability judgment accuracy: China, Japan, and the United States , 1989 .

[26]  M. Morris,et al.  Culture and Cause: American and Chinese Attributions for Social and Physical Events , 1994 .

[27]  J. Deręgowski Real space and represented space: Cross-cultural perspectives , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  E. Thelen Time-scale dynamics and the development of an embodied cognition , 1996 .

[29]  Robin Gregory,et al.  A Framework for Structuring Cross-Cultural Research in Risk and Decision Making , 1991 .

[30]  The moderating roles of self-esteem and self-construal in reaction to a threat to the self: evidence from the People's Republic of China and the United States. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  David C. McClelland,et al.  The Achieving Society. , 1964 .

[32]  E. Weber Who’s Afraid of a Little Risk? New Evidence for General Risk Aversion , 1999 .

[33]  Shawn P. Curley,et al.  Individual differences in risk taking. , 1992 .

[34]  Lawrence D. Phillips,et al.  CULTURAL VARIATION IN PROBABILISTIC THINKING: ALTERNATIVE WAYS OF DEALING WITH UNCERTAINTY* , 1980 .

[35]  Richard A. Shweder,et al.  Cultural psychology: Cultural psychology – what is it? , 1990 .

[36]  C. Palmer,et al.  Risk perception: an empirical study of the relationship between worldview and the risk construct. , 1996, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[37]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Trust, Emotion, Sex, Politics, and Science: Surveying the Risk-Assessment Battlefield , 1999 .

[38]  Branden B. Johnson,et al.  Risk and Culture Research , 1991 .

[39]  E. Weber,et al.  Perceived risk attitudes: relating risk perception to risky choice , 1997 .