Abstract Rule Learning for Visual Sequences in 8- and 11-Month-Olds.

The experiments reported here investigated the development of a fundamental component of cognition: to recognize and generalize abstract relations. Infants were presented with simple rule-governed patterned sequences of visual shapes (ABB, AAB, and ABA) that could be discriminated from differences in the position of the repeated element (late, early, or nonadjacent, respectively). Eight-month-olds were found to distinguish patterns on the basis of the repetition, but appeared insensitive to its position in the sequence; 11-month-olds distinguished patterns over the position of the repetition, but appeared insensitive to the nonadjacent repetition. These results suggest that abstract pattern detection may develop incrementally in a process of constructing complex relations from more primitive components.

[1]  William M. Smith,et al.  A Study of Thinking , 1956 .

[2]  R. Brown,et al.  A First Language , 1973 .

[3]  Bertram C. Bruce,et al.  Theoretical issues in reading comprehension : perspectives from cognitive psychology, linguistics, artificial intelligence, and education , 1980 .

[4]  M. Bornstein Habituation of attention as a measure of visual information processing in human infants: Summary , 1985 .

[5]  L B Cohen,et al.  Developmental change in infants' perception of correlations among attributes. , 1986, Child development.

[6]  A. Leslie,et al.  Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? , 1987, Cognition.

[7]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[8]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[9]  Donald J. Tyrrell,et al.  Perception of abstract identity/difference relationships by infants , 1991 .

[10]  I. Biederman,et al.  Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. , 1992, Psychological review.

[11]  J. Kruschke,et al.  ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.

[12]  U. Goswami Analogical reasoning in children , 1993 .

[13]  Donald J. Tyrrell,et al.  Learning and transfer of identity-difference relationships by infants* , 1993 .

[14]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Perception and 24-Hour Retention of Feature Relations in Infancy. , 1994 .

[15]  S. Pinker The Language Instinct , 1994 .

[16]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[17]  L. Cohen,et al.  Precursors to infants' perception of the causality of a simple event , 1998 .

[18]  Carolyn Rovee-Collier,et al.  Memory Processing of a Serial List by Young Infants , 1998 .

[19]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[20]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[21]  A. Churchland,et al.  Early developments in the ability to understand the relation between stimulus and reward. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[22]  Alan Slater,et al.  Intermodal perception at birth: Intersensory redundancy guides newborn infants’ learning of arbitrary auditory−visual pairings , 1999 .

[23]  R. Gómez,et al.  Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge , 1999, Cognition.

[24]  B. Roder,et al.  Infants' Preferences for Familiarity and Novelty During the Course of Visual Processing. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[25]  D. Gentner,et al.  The analogical mind : perspectives from cognitive science , 2001 .

[26]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Generalization, similarity, and Bayesian inference. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[27]  S. A. Rose,et al.  Visual short-term memory in the first year of life: capacity and recency effects. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[28]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.

[29]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[31]  R. Gómez Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.

[32]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[33]  S. Luck,et al.  The development of visual short-term memory capacity in infants. , 2003, Child development.

[34]  E. Newport,et al.  Learning at a distance I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  D. Lewkowicz Perception of serial order in infants. , 2004, Developmental science.

[36]  R. Gómez,et al.  The Developmental Trajectory of Nonadjacent Dependency Learning. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[37]  Daniel Swingley,et al.  Statistical clustering and the contents of the infant vocabulary , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  Jacques Mehler,et al.  The role of salience in the extraction of algebraic rules. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  Rebecca L. Gómez,et al.  Naps Promote Abstraction in Language-Learning Infants , 2006, Psychological science.

[41]  J. Saffran,et al.  Dog is a dog is a dog: Infant rule learning is not specific to language , 2007, Cognition.

[42]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Location, location, location: development of spatiotemporal sequence learning in infancy. , 2007, Child development.

[43]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Infant Rule Learning Facilitated by Speech , 2007, Psychological science.

[44]  The Acquisition of Expertise as a Model for the Growth of Cognitive Structure , 2009 .