Young children's understanding of markedness in non-verbal communication*

ABSTRACT Speakers often anticipate how recipients will interpret their utterances. If they wish some other, less obvious interpretation, they may ‘mark’ their utterance (e.g. with special intonations or facial expressions). We investigated whether two- and three-year-olds recognize when adults mark a non-verbal communicative act – in this case a pointing gesture – as special, and so search for a not-so-obvious referent. We set up the context of cleaning up and then pointed to an object. Three-year-olds inferred that the adult intended the pointing gesture to indicate that object, and so cleaned it up. However, when the adult marked her pointing gesture (with exaggerated facial expression) they took the object's hidden contents or a hidden aspect of it as the intended referent. Two-year-olds' appreciation of such marking was less clear-cut. These results demonstrate that markedness is not just a linguistic phenomenon, but rather something concerning the pragmatics of intentional communication more generally.

[1]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. , 2009, Developmental science.

[2]  Megan M. Saylor,et al.  Infants interpret ambiguous requests for absent objects. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[3]  E. Clark On the logic of contrast. , 1988, Journal of child language.

[4]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension , 2002 .

[5]  E. Battistella Markedness: The Evaluative Superstructure of Language , 1990 .

[6]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-Month-Olds Know What “We” Have Shared in a Special Way , 2008 .

[7]  M. Tomasello,et al.  One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. , 2005, Developmental science.

[8]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words* , 2007, Journal of Child Language.

[9]  Jennifer E. Arnold,et al.  THE BACON not the bacon: How children and adults understand accented and unaccented noun phrases , 2008, Cognition.

[10]  Julian M. Pine,et al.  Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. , 2004 .

[11]  Megan M. Saylor,et al.  Different kinds of information affect word learning in the preschool years: the case of part-term learning. , 2004, Child development.

[12]  E. Clark On the pragmatics of contrast , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[13]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  M. Tomasello,et al.  A new look at infant pointing. , 2007, Child development.

[15]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[16]  Megan M. Saylor,et al.  Infants' use of shared linguistic information to clarify ambiguous requests. , 2007, Child development.