Virtual Personalities: Using Computational Modeling to Understand Within-Person Variability.

How can the same underlying psychological/neurobiological system result in both stable between-individual differences and high levels of within-individual variability in personality states over time and situations? We argue that both types of variability result from a psychological system based on structured, chronic motivations, where behavior at a specific point in time is a joint function of the current availability of motive affordances in the situation, current motivationally relevant bodily or interoceptive states, and the result of the competition among alternative active motives. Here we present a biologically-based theoretical framework, embodied in two different computational models, that shows how individuals with stable personality characteristics, can nevertheless exhibit considerable within-person variability in personality states across time and situations.

[1]  W. Fleeson,et al.  What Is Extraversion For? Integrating Trait and Motivational Perspectives and Identifying the Purpose of Extraversion , 2012, Psychological science.

[2]  M. Ashton,et al.  Empirical, Theoretical, and Practical Advantages of the HEXACO Model of Personality Structure , 2007, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[3]  W. Mischel,et al.  Personality as a stable cognitive-affective activation network: Characteristic patterns of behavior variation emerge from a stable personality structure. , 1998 .

[4]  S. Read,et al.  On the Coherence of Mental Models of Persons and Relationships: A Knowledge Structure Approach , 2013 .

[5]  Nicolas A. Brown,et al.  The independent effects of personality and situations on real-time expressions of behavior and emotion. , 2015, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  S. Read,et al.  A neural network model of the structure and dynamics of human personality. , 2010, Psychological review.

[7]  Ravi Iyer,et al.  Toward a comprehensive taxonomy of human motives , 2017, PloS one.

[8]  J. Gray,et al.  Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  W. Fleeson,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Are Traits Useful ? Explaining Trait Manifestations as Tools in the Pursuit of Goals , 2016 .

[10]  A. Bechara,et al.  Listening to your heart: interoceptive awareness as a gateway to feeling , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  Douglas T. Kenrick,et al.  Evolutionary approaches to relationships , 1997 .

[12]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  From Experienced Utility to Decision Utility , 2014 .

[13]  杨文秀,et al.  此处“personality”译法探析 , 2000 .

[14]  Stephen J. Read,et al.  Traits as Goal-Based Categories: The Importance of Goals in the Coherence of Dispositional Categories , 1990 .

[15]  S. Read,et al.  Virtual Personalities: A Neural Network Model of Personality , 2007 .

[16]  Matthias Ziegler,et al.  The Situational Eight DIAMONDS: a taxonomy of major dimensions of situation characteristics. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  P. Corr,et al.  The neuropsychology of fear and anxiety: A foundation for Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. , 2008 .

[18]  D. Bugental,et al.  Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: a domain-based approach. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  O. John,et al.  Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. , 2008 .

[20]  F. Richard Ferraro,et al.  Handbook of Approach and Avoidance Motivation , 2010 .

[21]  W. Fleeson Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: traits as density distribution of states. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  K. Berridge Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience , 2004, Physiology & Behavior.

[23]  S. Read,et al.  Revisiting the role of the insula in addiction , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  W. Revelle,et al.  A model for personality at three levels , 2015 .

[25]  S. Read,et al.  Why Am I Telling You This , 1987 .

[26]  S. Read,et al.  A Hierarchical Taxonomy of Human Goals , 2001 .

[27]  C. DeYoung Cybernetic Big Five Theory. , 2015 .

[28]  K. Berridge From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  W. Fleeson,et al.  Whole Trait Theory. , 2015, Journal of research in personality.

[30]  Brian Mingus,et al.  The Emergent neural modeling system , 2008, Neural Networks.

[31]  D. Ozer,et al.  Goal conflict, goal striving, and psychological well-being , 2012, Motivation and Emotion.

[32]  Stephen J. Read,et al.  Inter-personalism: Toward a goal-based theory of persons in relationships. , 1989 .

[33]  J. W. Atkinson,et al.  On the dynamics of action. , 1970, Nederlands tijdschrift voor de psychologie en haar grensgebieden.

[34]  C. Carver,et al.  On the Self-Regulation of Behavior , 1998 .

[35]  C. Carver Approach, Avoidance, and the Self-Regulation of Affect and Action , 2006 .

[36]  W. Mischel,et al.  A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. , 1995, Psychological review.

[37]  Patrick Gallagher,et al.  The implications of Big Five standing for the distribution of trait manifestation in behavior: fifteen experience-sampling studies and a meta-analysis. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  M. Ashton,et al.  Psychometric Properties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory , 2004, Multivariate behavioral research.

[39]  C. Carver,et al.  Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales , 1994 .