Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task.

The error negativity (Ne/ERN) and error positivity (Pe) are two components of the event-related brain potential (ERP) that are associated with action monitoring and error detection. To investigate the relation between error processing and conscious self-monitoring of behavior, the present experiment examined whether an Ne and Pe are observed after response errors of which participants are unaware. Ne and Pe measures, behavioral accuracy, and trial-to-trial subjective accuracy judgments were obtained from participants performing an antisaccade task, which elicits many unperceived, incorrect reflex-like saccades. Consistent with previous research, subjectively unperceived saccade errors were almost always immediately corrected, and were associated with faster correction times and smaller saccade sizes than perceived errors. Importantly, irrespective of whether the participant was aware of the error or not, erroneous saccades were followed by a sizable Ne. In contrast, the Pe was much more pronounced for perceived than for unperceived errors. Unperceived errors were characterized by the absence of posterror slowing. These and other results are consistent with the view that the Ne and Pe reflect the activity of two separate error monitoring processes, of which only the later process, reflected by the Pe, is associated with conscious error recognition and remedial action.

[1]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  P M Rabbitt,et al.  Three Kinds of Error-Signalling Responses in a Serial Choice Task , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[4]  L E Mays,et al.  Saccades are spatially, not retinocentrically, coded. , 1980, Science.

[5]  J. C. Woestenburg,et al.  The removal of the eye-movement artifact from the EEG by regression analysis in the frequency domain , 1983, Biological Psychology.

[6]  Virginia A. Diggles,et al.  Rapid error correction during human arm movements: evidence for central monitoring. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[9]  R. Roberts,et al.  Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. , 1994 .

[10]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[11]  M. Coles,et al.  "Where did I go wrong?" A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  G. Karmos,et al.  Perspectives of Event-Related Potentials Research , 1995 .

[13]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of errors in reaction tasks. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[14]  E. Donchin,et al.  Event-related brain potentials and error-related processing: an analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[15]  J. Kaiser,et al.  Hypnosis and event-related potential correlates of error processing in a stroop-type paradigm: a test of the frontal hypothesis. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[16]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Error-related scalp potentials elicited by hand and foot movements: evidence for an output-independent error-processing system in humans , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[18]  D. Munoz,et al.  Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[20]  B. Fischer,et al.  The recognition and correction of involuntary prosaccades in an antisaccade task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  A F Kramer,et al.  Error-related processing during a period of extended wakefulness. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[22]  P. Apkarian,et al.  Motoric response inhibition in finger movement and saccadic eye movement: a comparative study , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  F. Vidal,et al.  Is the ‘error negativity’ specific to errors? , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[24]  D. Tucker,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex in Action Monitoring , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  D. Tucker,et al.  Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error monitoring. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Age Differences in the Control of Looking Behavior: Do You Know Where Your Eyes Have Been? , 2000, Psychological science.

[27]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[28]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[29]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  M. Coles,et al.  Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  A Kok,et al.  Inhibitory inefficiency and failures of intention activation: age-related decline in the control of saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Psychology and aging.