The Interaction of Spatial and Object Pathways: Evidence from Balint's Syndrome

An earlier report described a patient (RM) with bilateral parietal damage who showed severe binding problems between shape and color and shape and size (Friedman-Hill, Robertson, & Treisman, 1995). When shown two different-colored letters, RM reported a large number of illusory conjunctions (ICs) combining the shape of one letter with the color of the other, even when he was looking directly at one of them and had as long as 10 sec to respond. The lesions also produced severe deficits in locating and reaching for objects, and difficulty in seeing more than one object at a time, resulting in a neuropsychological diagnosis of Balint's syndrome or dorsal simultanagnosia. The pattern of deficits supported predictions of Treisman's Feature Integration Theory (FIT) that the loss of spatial information would lead to binding errors. They further suggested that the spatial information used in binding depends on intact parietal function. In the present paper we extend these findings and examine other deficits in RM that would be predicted by FIT. We show that: (1) Object individuation is impaired, making it impossible for him correctly to count more than one or two objects, even when he is aware that more are present. (2) Visual search for a target defined by a conjunction of features (requiring binding) is impaired, while the detection of a target defined by a unique feature is not. Search for the absence of a feature (0 among Qs) is also severely impaired, while search for the presence (Q among 0s) is not. Feature absence can only be detected when all the present features are bound to the nontarget items. (3) RM's deficits cannot be attributed to a general binding problem: binding errors were far more likely with simultaneous presentation where spatial information was required than with sequential presentation where time could be used as the medium for binding. (4) Selection for attention was severely impaired, whether it was based on the position of a marker or on some other feature (color). (5) Spatial information seems to exist that RM cannot access, suggesting that feature binding relies on a relatively late stage where implicit spatial information is made explicitly accessible. The data converge to support our conclusions that explicit spatial knowledge is necessary for the perception of accurately bound features, for accurate attentional selection, and for accurate and rapid search for a conjunction of features in a multiitem display. It is obviously necessary for directing attention to spatial locations, but the consequences of impairments in this ability seem also to affect object selection, object individuation, and feature integration. Thus, the functional effects of parietal damage are not limited to the spatial and attentional problems that have long been described in patients with Balint's syndrome. Damage to parietal areas also affects object perception through damage to spatial representations that are fundamental for spatial awareness.

[1]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 51–66 , 1909 .

[2]  G Holmes,et al.  DISTURBANCES OF VISUAL ORIENTATION , 1918, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[3]  G. Holmes,et al.  DISTURBANCES OF SPATIAL ORIENTATION AND VISUAL ATTENTION, WITH LOSS OF STEREOSCOPIC VISION , 1919 .

[4]  K. Duncker,et al.  Über induzierte Bewegung , 1929 .

[5]  Edwin G. Boring,et al.  The Perception of Objects , 1946 .

[6]  A. Luria,et al.  Disorders of "simultaneous perception" in a case of bilateral occipito-parietal brain injury. , 1959, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  C. McCollough Color Adaptation of Edge-Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1965, Science.

[8]  W. Ritchie Russell,et al.  Dissociated visual perceptual and spatial deficits in focal lesions of the right hemisphere , 1969 .

[9]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[12]  A. Treisman,et al.  Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  A. Marcel Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  N. Mai,et al.  Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of visual attention , 1984 .

[18]  A. Treisman,et al.  Emergent features, attention, and object perception. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[20]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[21]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Attention and Priming: Inhibitory and Facilitatory Effects of Ignored Primes , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits for Spatial Attention and Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .

[23]  S W Keele,et al.  Tests of a temporal theory of attentional binding. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  A. Treisman Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[25]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[26]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[27]  Anne Treisman,et al.  Visual Information-Processing in the Perception of Features and Objects , 1988 .

[28]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  A. Treisman Search, similarity, and integration of features between and within dimensions. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  C. Colby The neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of attention. , 1991, Journal of child neurology.

[32]  H. Coslett,et al.  Simultanagnosia. To see but not two see. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  R. Rafal,et al.  Attention and Feature Integration: Illusory Conjunctions in a Patient with a Parietal Lobe Lesion , 1991 .

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  Elpeleg On,et al.  Hereditary cerebellar hypoplasia. , 1991, Journal of child neurology.

[36]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Interactions between object and space systems revealed through neuropsychology , 1993 .

[39]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Jon Driver,et al.  Reading of letters and words in a patient with Balint's syndrome , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  C. Gross,et al.  Mapping Space With Neurons , 1994 .

[42]  E. Marg A VISION OF THE BRAIN , 1994 .

[43]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Visual Feature Integration with an Attention Deficit , 1994, Brain and Cognition.

[44]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Non-spatial extinction following lesions of the parietal lobe in humans , 1994, Nature.

[45]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  On Neural Codes and Perception , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  The handbook of brain theory and neural networks , 1995, A Bradford book.

[48]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[49]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  The neurology of visual attention. , 1995 .

[50]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The McCollough effect reveals orientation discrimination in a case of cortical blindness , 1995, Current Biology.

[51]  R. Ivry,et al.  Loosening the constraints on illusory conjunctions: assessing the roles of exposure duration and attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  A. Treisman,et al.  Parietal contributions to visual feature binding: evidence from a patient with bilateral lesions , 1995, Science.

[53]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[54]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[55]  R Ivry,et al.  A formal theory of feature binding in object perception. , 1996, Psychological review.

[56]  A. Treisman,et al.  Perceiving visually presented objets: recognition, awareness, and modularity , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.