Eye movements in neurodevelopmental disorders

Purpose of reviewThe aim of this paper is to review the literature on eye-movement abnormalities associated with neurodevelopmental disorders. Eye-movement testing is a non-invasive quantitative approach for evaluating brain systems across the age spectrum. It thus provides a promising methodology for characterizing and documenting maturational abnormalities in brain systems associated with neurodevelopmental disorders. Recent findingsRecent oculomotor studies have made significant contributions to the understanding of neurodevelopmental disorders, most notably in autism, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and Tourette's syndrome. Notably different patterns of deficits have been found in these disorders and have helped to clarify their pathophysiology. SummaryEye-movement studies have begun to serve as a useful approach for studying cognitive and neurophysiological aspects of neurodevelopmental disorders. They also have potential as a strategy for establishing quantitative endophenotypes for genetic research, and for monitoring beneficial and adverse effects of pharmacotherapies. Studies are needed that involve larger patient populations, longitudinal characterization of developmental failures, patients free from central nervous system-active medications, and that use functional imaging, as patients perform eye-movement tasks, for direct identification of clinically relevant abnormalities in brain systems.

[1]  N. Minshew,et al.  Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism , 1999, Neurology.

[2]  J. Piven,et al.  Visual Scanning of Faces in Autism , 2002, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[3]  C. Klein,et al.  Development of prosaccade and antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to 26 years. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[4]  D. Zee,et al.  Evidence of Normal Cerebellar Control of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex (VOR) in Children with High-Functioning Autism , 2000, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[5]  D. Munoz,et al.  Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Ruth A. Carper,et al.  Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder , 2001, Neurology.

[8]  D. Hommer,et al.  Altered performance on an ocular fixation task in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[9]  Burkhart Fischer,et al.  On the development of voluntary and reflexive components in human saccade generation , 1997, Brain Research.

[10]  C. Klein,et al.  Effects of methylphenidate on saccadic responses in patients with ADHD , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  D. Munoz,et al.  Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  G. Dawson,et al.  Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder , 2002, Neurology.

[13]  N. Minshew,et al.  Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder , 1997, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[14]  Yun Zhou,et al.  Elevated intrasynaptic dopamine release in Tourette's syndrome measured by PET. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[15]  A. Bailey,et al.  A clinicopathological study of autism. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  P. Renshaw,et al.  Volumetric MRI analysis comparing subjects having attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with normal controls , 1997, Neurology.

[17]  D. Zee,et al.  Oculomotor abnormalities in boys with tourette syndrome with and without ADHD. , 2001, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[18]  D. Munoz,et al.  Altered control of visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  N. Minshew,et al.  Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory , 2002, Neurology.

[22]  Godfrey D Pearlson,et al.  MRI parcellation of the frontal lobe in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder or Tourette syndrome , 2002, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[23]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Control of volitional and reflexive saccades in Tourette's syndrome. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[24]  S. Dursun,et al.  Antisaccade eye movement abnormalities in Tourette syndrome: evidence for cortico-striatal network dysfunction? , 2000, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[25]  D. Zee,et al.  Oculomotor abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. A preliminary study.1 , 2002 .

[26]  P. Maruff,et al.  Contextual abnormalities of saccadic inhibition in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  N. Minshew,et al.  Maturation of Widely Distributed Brain Function Subserves Cognitive Development , 2001, NeuroImage.

[28]  C. Klein,et al.  Development of pro- and antisaccades in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[29]  M. Ernst,et al.  High presynaptic dopaminergic activity in children with Tourette's disorder. , 1999, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[30]  Chantal Kemner,et al.  Eye movements, visual attention, and autism: a saccadic reaction time study using the gap and overlap paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[31]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. , 2002, Archives of general psychiatry.

[32]  D. Zee,et al.  Deficits in the initiation of eye movements in the absence of a visual target in adolescents with high functioning autism , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Control of volitional and reflexive saccades in Tourette's syndrome. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Saccadic performance characteristics and the behavioural neurology of Tourette’s syndrome , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[35]  Heping Zhang,et al.  Basal Ganglia volumes in patients with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. , 2003, Archives of general psychiatry.

[36]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[37]  Andrew E. Switala,et al.  Minicolumnar pathology in autism , 2002, Neurology.

[38]  Y. Samson,et al.  Delayed maturation of the frontal cortex in childhood autism. , 1995, The American journal of psychiatry.

[39]  R. Joseph,et al.  Holistic and part-based face recognition in children with autism. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[40]  H. Engeland,et al.  Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: a fixation time study. , 2002, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[41]  L. Colgin,et al.  Society for Neuroscience , 2005, Nature.

[42]  D. Munoz,et al.  Inhibitory control of eye movements during oculomotor countermanding in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.