Binding Action and Emotion in Social Understanding

In social life actions are tightly linked with emotions. The integration of affective- and action-related information has to be considered as a fundamental component of appropriate social understanding. The present functional magnetic resonance imaging study aimed at investigating whether an emotion (Happiness, Anger or Neutral) dynamically expressed by an observed agent modulates brain activity underlying the perception of his grasping action. As control stimuli, participants observed the same agent either only expressing an emotion or only performing a grasping action. Our results showed that the observation of an action embedded in an emotional context (agent’s facial expression), compared with the observation of the same action embedded in a neutral context, elicits higher neural response at the level of motor frontal cortices, temporal and occipital cortices, bilaterally. Particularly, the dynamic facial expression of anger modulates the re-enactment of a motor representation of the observed action. This is supported by the evidence that observing actions embedded in the context of anger, but not happiness, compared with a neutral context, elicits stronger activity in the bilateral pre-central gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus, besides the pre-supplementary motor area, a region playing a central role in motor control. Angry faces not only seem to modulate the simulation of actions, but may also trigger motor reaction. These findings suggest that emotions exert a modulatory role on action observation in different cortical areas involved in action processing.

[1]  J. Stainer,et al.  The Emotions , 1922, Nature.

[2]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  J. R. Augustine Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans , 1996, Brain Research Reviews.

[7]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[8]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[9]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  N. Sadato,et al.  Attention to emotion modulates fMRI activity in human right superior temporal sulcus. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[11]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  J C Mazziotta,et al.  Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Cortical Motor System , 2001, Neuron.

[14]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[15]  C. Babiloni,et al.  Influence of the supplementary motor area on primary motor cortex excitability during movements triggered by neutral or emotionally unpleasant visual cues , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling Disgust , 2003, Neuron.

[17]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Actions or Hand-Object Interactions? Human Inferior Frontal Cortex and Action Observation , 2003, Neuron.

[19]  E. Naito,et al.  Enhanced neural activity in response to dynamic facial expressions of emotion: an fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  C. Don Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain , 2004 .

[21]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Underlying Imitation Learning of Hand Actions An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2004, Neuron.

[22]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use , 2004, NeuroImage.

[23]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[24]  Rüdiger J Seitz,et al.  Functional modularity of the medial prefrontal cortex: involvement in human empathy. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[25]  Hugo D Critchley,et al.  Imitating expressions: emotion-specific neural substrates in facial mimicry. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[26]  T. Paus,et al.  Brain networks involved in viewing angry hands or faces. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Goal Representation in Human Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  C. Keysers,et al.  Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex , 2007, NeuroImage.

[30]  Aina Puce,et al.  Common and distinct brain activation to viewing dynamic sequences of face and hand movements , 2007, NeuroImage.

[31]  Jejo D. Koola,et al.  Emotion facilitates action: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study of motor cortex excitability during picture viewing. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[32]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Observing social gestures: an fMRI study , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  J. Grèzes,et al.  Emotional modulation of visual and motor areas by dynamic body expressions of anger , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[34]  Gaspare Galati,et al.  A selective representation of the meaning of actions in the auditory mirror system , 2008, NeuroImage.

[35]  Roel M. Willems,et al.  Complementary Systems for Understanding Action Intentions , 2008, Current Biology.

[36]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  J. Grèzes,et al.  What is “mirror” in the premotor cortex? A review , 2008, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[38]  Frank E. Pollick,et al.  Neural Substrates for Action Understanding at Different Description Levels in the Human Brain , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  C. Keysers,et al.  A Common Anterior Insula Representation of Disgust Observation, Experience and Imagination Shows Divergent Functional Connectivity Pathways , 2008, PloS one.

[40]  C. Keysers,et al.  Action Understanding : How , What and Why , 2022 .

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Further reflections on how we interpret the actions of others , 2008, Nature.

[42]  Swann Pichon,et al.  Two different faces of threat. Comparing the neural systems for recognizing fear and anger in dynamic body expressions , 2009, NeuroImage.

[43]  Julie Grèzes,et al.  Specific and common brain regions involved in the perception of faces and bodies and the representation of their emotional expressions , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[44]  G. Fink,et al.  Neurofunctional modulation of brain regions by distinct forms of motor cognition and movement features , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[45]  David M. Sobel,et al.  Action Understanding in the Superior Temporal Sulcus Region , 2009, Psychological science.

[46]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Motor abstraction: a neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood , 2009, Psychological research.

[47]  J. Summers,et al.  Emotion and motor preparation: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of corticospinal motor tract excitability , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[48]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  Gaspare Galati,et al.  Intentional signals during saccadic and reaching delays in the human posterior parietal cortex , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[50]  V. Gallese,et al.  What is so special about embodied simulation? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  C. Heyes Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[52]  I. Koch,et al.  The role of emotional context in facilitating imitative actions. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[53]  V. Gallese,et al.  How the body in action shapes the self , 2011 .

[54]  James M. Kilner,et al.  More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Gabriele K. Lünser,et al.  Automatic imitation in a strategic context: players of rock–paper–scissors imitate opponents' gestures† , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[56]  P. Enticott,et al.  Emotional valence modulates putative mirror neuron activity , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[57]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Activation patterns during action observation are modulated by context in mirror system areas , 2012, NeuroImage.