Dutch norm data for 13 semantic categories and 338 exemplars

A data set is described that includes eight variables gathered for 13 common superordinate natural language categories and a representative set of 338 exemplars in Dutch. The category set contains 6 animal categories (reptiles, amphibians, mammals, birds, fish, andinsects), 3 artifact categories (musical instruments, tools, andvehicles), 2 borderline artifact-natural-kind categories (vegetables andfruit), and 2 activity categories (sports andprofessions). In an exemplar and a feature generation task for the category nouns, frequency data were collected. For each of the 13 categories, a representative sample of 5–30 exemplars was selected. For all exemplars, feature generation frequencies, typicality ratings, pairwise similarity ratings, age-of-acquisition ratings, word frequencies, and word associations were gathered. Reliability estimates and some additional measures are presented. The full set of these norms is available in Excel format at the Psychonomic Society Web archive,www.psychonomic. org/archive/.

[1]  M Brysbaert,et al.  Age-of-acquisition effects in semantic processing tasks. , 2000, Acta psychologica.

[2]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  A validation study of the age-of-acquisition norms collected by Ghyselinck, De Moor, & Brysbaert , 2000 .

[3]  Gert Storms,et al.  Linear separability in superordinate natural language concepts , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[4]  P. Boeck,et al.  The instantiation principle re-evaluated , 2003, Memory.

[5]  M. Gernsbacher Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Word age-of-acquisition, reading latencies and auditory recognition , 1981 .

[7]  Gert Storms Wim Ruts Anneloes Vandenbroucke Dominance, Overextensions and the Conjunction Effect in Different Syntactic Phrasings of Concept Conjunctions , 1998 .

[8]  R. Burchfield Frequency Analysis of English Usage: Lexicon and Grammar. By W. Nelson Francis and Henry Kučera with the assistance of Andrew W. Mackie. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 1982. x + 561 , 1985 .

[9]  F. Keil,et al.  Early differentiation of causal mechanisms appropriate to biological and nonbiological kinds. , 1991, Child development.

[10]  J. Fleiss,et al.  Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  W. A. Bousfield,et al.  CULTURAL NORMS FOR VERBAL ITEMS IN 43 CATEGORIES , 1957 .

[12]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  The effects of age-of-acquisition and frequency-of-occurrence in visual word recognition: Further evidence from the Dutch language , 2000 .

[13]  R. Logie,et al.  Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words , 1980 .

[14]  Gerrit Storms,et al.  Prototype and Exemplar-Based Information in Natural Language Categories , 2000 .

[15]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  First in, first out: Word learning age and spoken word frequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[16]  J. Hampton Polymorphous Concepts in Semantic Memory , 1979 .

[17]  G. Storms,et al.  The Role of Contrast Categories in Natural Language Concepts , 2001 .

[18]  G. Kellas,et al.  Typicality as a Dimension of Encoding. , 1978 .

[19]  Tim Smits,et al.  Fruits and vegetables categorized: An application of the generalized context model , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  Curt Burgess,et al.  The Dynamics of Meaning in Memory , 1998 .

[21]  K. Forster,et al.  Lexical Access and Naming Time. , 1973 .

[22]  Serge Larochelle,et al.  Determinants of Response Times in the Semantic Verification Task , 1994 .

[23]  Gert Storms,et al.  Categorization of novel stimuli in well-known natural concepts: A case study , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  Thomas A. Schreiber,et al.  The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[25]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Correlated properties in natural categories , 1984 .

[26]  A. M. B. De Groot Woordassociatienormen met reactietijden , 1988 .

[27]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Age-of-acquisition ratings for 2332 Dutch words from 49 different semantic categories , 2003 .

[29]  Douglas L. Nelson,et al.  Category Name and Instance Norms for 106 Categories of Various Sizes , 1982 .

[30]  Iven Van Mechelen,et al.  THE DOMINANCE EFFECT IN CONCEPT CONJUNCTIONS : GENERALITY AND INTERACTION ASPECTS , 1996 .

[31]  Gerrit Storms Flemish Category Norms for Exemplars of 39 Categories: A Replication of the Battig and Montague (1969) Category Norms , 2001 .

[32]  A. Paivio,et al.  Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  Barbara C. Malt,et al.  Do artifact concepts have cores , 1992 .

[34]  C. P. Whaley Word–nonword classification time. , 1978 .

[35]  James A. Hampton,et al.  Measures of internal category structure: A correlational analysis of normative data , 1983 .

[36]  E. Heit,et al.  The instantiation principle in natural categories. , 1996, Memory.

[37]  Glyn W Humphreys,et al.  A CASE SERIES ANALYSIS OF “CATEGORY-SPECIFIC” DEFICITS OF LIVING THINGS:THE HIT ACCOUNT , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[38]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Category-Based Induction , 1990 .

[39]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Age of Acquisition Norms for a Large Set of Object Names and Their Relation to Adult Estimates and Other Variables , 1997 .

[40]  R. Nosofsky American Psychological Association, Inc. Choice, Similarity, and the Context Theory of Classification , 2022 .

[41]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[42]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations , 1973 .

[43]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[44]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Hierarchies, similarity, and interactivity in object recognition: “Category-specific” neuropsychological deficits , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[45]  Ken McRae,et al.  Category - Specific semantic deficits , 2008 .

[46]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Thirty categorization results in search of a model. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  S. Gerhand,et al.  Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the role of phonology in the lexical decision task , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[48]  P. Groenen,et al.  Modern Multidimensional Scaling: Theory and Applications , 1999 .

[49]  L. Rips Inductive judgments about natural categories. , 1975 .

[50]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[51]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  The effect of age of acquisition in visual word processing: further evidence for the semantic hypothesis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[53]  Robert D. Rodman,et al.  An Introduction to Language , 1984 .

[54]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Natural categories: Well defined or fuzzy sets? , 1978 .

[55]  E. Rosch Cognitive reference points , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[56]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[57]  W. Montague,et al.  Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms , 1969 .