Dorsal and Ventral Stream Activation and Object Recognition Performance in School-age Children

We explored how developing neural artifact and animal representations in the dorsal and ventral stream play a role in children's increasingly more proficient interactions with objects. In thirty-three 6- to 10-year-old children and 11 adults, we used fMRI to track the development of (1) the cortical category preference for tools compared to animals and (2) the response to complex objects (as compared to scrambled objects) during a passive viewing task. In addition, we related a cognitive skill that improved substantially from age 6 to 10, namely the ability to recognize tools from unusual viewpoints, to the development of cortical object processing. In multiple complementary analyses we showed that those children who were better at recognizing tools from unusual viewpoints outside the scanner showed a reduced cortical response to tools and animals when viewed inside the scanner, bilaterally in intraparietal and inferotemporal cortex. In contrast, the cortical preference for tools in the dorsal and ventral visual stream did not predict object recognition performance, and was organized in an adult-like manner at six. While cortical tool preference did not change with age, the findings suggest that animal-preferring regions in the ventral visual stream may develop later, concordant with previous reports of a protracted development in similar regions for faces. We thus conclude that intraparietal and inferotemporal cortical networks that support aspects of object processing irrespective of tool or animal category, continue to develop during the school-age years and contribute to the development of object recognition skills during this period.

[1]  E. Darcy Burgund,et al.  Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space , 2003, NeuroImage.

[2]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Differential development of selectivity for faces and bodies in the fusiform gyrus. , 2009, Developmental science.

[3]  R. Poldrack Interpreting developmental changes in neuroimaging signals , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[4]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Action-Related Properties Shape Object Representations in the Ventral Stream , 2007, Neuron.

[6]  S. Kastner,et al.  Two hierarchically organized neural systems for object information in human visual cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Development of object recognition in humans , 2009, F1000 biology reports.

[8]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[9]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  Mark H. Johnson Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3rd Edition , 2010 .

[11]  Cristina Montomoli,et al.  The Development of Visual Object Recognition in School-Age Children , 2007, Developmental neuropsychology.

[12]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Précis of Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  Sarah Durston,et al.  A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. , 2006, Developmental science.

[14]  Benjamin Y. Hayden,et al.  Posterior Cingulate Cortex Mediates Outcome-Contingent Allocation of Behavior , 2008, Neuron.

[15]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  K. Grill-Spector The neural basis of object perception , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[17]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[18]  L. Schauble,et al.  Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. , 1994 .

[19]  J. Haxby,et al.  fMRI Responses to Video and Point-Light Displays of Moving Humans and Manipulable Objects , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  U-Shaped Interest in U-Shaped Development-and What It Means , 2004 .

[21]  Philip N. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Thinking; Readings in Cognitive Science , 1977 .

[22]  Flavio M. de Paula,et al.  Developmental differences in the neural bases of the face inversion effect show progressive tuning of face-selective regions to the upright orientation , 2007, NeuroImage.

[23]  Ernst Martin,et al.  Dorsal stream development in motion and structure-from-motion perception , 2008, NeuroImage.

[24]  Elinor McKone,et al.  Early maturity of face recognition: No childhood development of holistic processing, novel face encoding, or face-space , 2009, Cognition.

[25]  Kathrin Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Developing a cortex specialized for face perception , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  Peter Fransson,et al.  The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis , 2008, NeuroImage.

[27]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Temporal Autocorrelation in Univariate Linear Modeling of FMRI Data , 2001, NeuroImage.

[28]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[29]  O. Muzik,et al.  Statistical Parametric Mapping: Assessment of Application in Children , 2000, NeuroImage.

[30]  B. Inhelder,et al.  If you want to get ahead, get a theory , 1975, Cognition.

[31]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Mark H. Johnson Functional brain development in humans , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[33]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Increased Brain Activity in Frontal and Parietal Cortex Underlies the Development of Visuospatial Working Memory Capacity during Childhood , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  L. Vygotsky Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes: Harvard University Press , 1978 .

[35]  Pierre Mounoud,et al.  The influence of action perception on object recognition: a developmental study. , 2007, Developmental science.

[36]  Jody C. Culham,et al.  Does tool-related fMRI activity within the intraparietal sulcus reflect the plan to grasp? , 2007, NeuroImage.

[37]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. , 2007, Developmental science.

[38]  J. Bruner,et al.  Studies In Cognitive Growth , 1966 .

[39]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Shape selectivity in primate lateral intraparietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[40]  S. Carey Becoming a face expert. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[41]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  L. Vygotsky,et al.  Tool and symbol in child development , 2008 .

[43]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.

[44]  M. Arbib From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[45]  J. R. Augustine Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans , 1996, Brain Research Reviews.

[46]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Action-related properties of objects shape object representations in the ventral stream , 2007 .

[48]  D. Maurer,et al.  Configural Face Processing Develops more Slowly than Featural Face Processing , 2002, Perception.

[49]  R. Milo,et al.  A central role for Necl4 (SynCAM4) in Schwann cell–axon interaction and myelination , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’ , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Developmental Continuity and Change in Responses to Social and Nonsocial Categories in Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[52]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Mapping functional brain development: Building a social brain through interactive specialization. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[55]  Martin Jüttner,et al.  A developmental dissociation of view-dependent and view-invariant object recognition in adolescence , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[56]  Jane E Joseph,et al.  Progressive and regressive developmental changes in neural substrates for face processing: testing specific predictions of the Interactive Specialization account. , 2011, Developmental science.

[57]  Monique Ernst,et al.  Developmental differences in neuronal engagement during implicit encoding of emotional faces: an event-related fMRI study. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[58]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Category-Specific Organization in the Human Brain Does Not Require Visual Experience , 2009, Neuron.

[59]  S Thesen,et al.  Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image‐based tracking for real‐time fMRI , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[60]  Jennifer M. D. Yoon,et al.  Differential development of the ventral visual cortex extends through adolescence , 2010 .

[61]  J E Joseph,et al.  Developmental shifts in cortical loci for face and object recognition , 2004, Neuroreport.

[62]  M. Sereno,et al.  Retinotopy and Attention in Human Occipital, Temporal, Parietal, and Frontal Cortex , 2008 .

[63]  E. Bruce Goldstein,et al.  Encyclopedia of perception , 2010 .

[64]  R. Buxton,et al.  The development of face and location processing: an fMRI study , 2003 .

[65]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Detecting and adjusting for artifacts in fMRI time series data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[66]  Evan M. Gordon,et al.  Developmental cognitive neuroscience , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[68]  S. Petersen,et al.  The maturing architecture of the brain's default network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[69]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[70]  L. S. Vygotskiĭ,et al.  Mind in society : the development of higher psychological processes , 1978 .

[71]  A. Reiss,et al.  Brain development, gender and IQ in children. A volumetric imaging study. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[72]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[73]  D. Maurer,et al.  Developmental changes in face processing skills. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[74]  Rafael Malach,et al.  Large-Scale Mirror-Symmetry Organization of Human Occipito-Temporal Object Areas , 2003, Neuron.