Dissociation between manipulation and conceptual knowledge of object use in the supramarginalis gyrus

Because tool naming activates motor‐related areas in the posterior parietal cortex, it has been suggested that conceptual knowledge of tools relies on their unique manipulation patterns. However, this view is questioned by the finding that some patients impaired in retrieving manipulation knowledge of man‐made objects are still able to perform conceptual judgments on them. To address this issue, we used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to interfere with the functioning of the anterior part of the right or left supramarginalis gyrus (SMG), a region critically involved in object‐directed actions. rTMS was delivered in healthy participants performing four judgment tasks designed to explore different aspects of manipulation and conceptual knowledge of man‐made objects. The two manipulation judgment tasks consisted in determining whether (1) two objects displayed on a computer screen are normally used by adopting a comparable hand posture, or (2) a given hand posture is appropriate to use an object. In the two conceptual judgment tasks, subjects had to decide whether (1) two objects displayed on the computer screen are normally used in the same context or (2) they are functionally related. We found that virtual lesions of left SMG interfere only with the performance of the manipulation judgment task in which subjects had to decide whether two different objects are used by adopting the same hand posture, all the other tasks being unaltered. rTMS applied over the right SMG had no effect. These results challenge the assumption that conceptual knowledge of tools is grounded upon motor representations. Hum Brain Mapp, 2011. © 2010 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  R. Ivry,et al.  Ipsilateral motor cortex activity during unimanual hand movements relates to task complexity. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  H. Sakata,et al.  Selectivity for the shape, size, and orientation of objects for grasping in neurons of monkey parietal area AIP. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  Guy Vingerhoets,et al.  Knowing about tools: Neural correlates of tool familiarity and experience , 2008, NeuroImage.

[7]  Eugene Tunik,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation to the Frontal Operculum and Supramarginal Gyrus Disrupts Planning of Outcome-Based Hand–Object Interactions , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[9]  M. Brett,et al.  Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Motor imagery while judging object–hand interactions , 2005, Neuroreport.

[11]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[12]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[15]  G. Gigli,et al.  Degraded Semantic Knowledge And Accurate Object Use , 2007, Cortex.

[16]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  J. Culham,et al.  The human dorsal stream adapts to real actions and 3D shape processing: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Simona Luzzi,et al.  Semantic memory is an amodal, dynamic system: Evidence from the interaction of naming and object use in semantic dementia , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[19]  Michael Andres,et al.  Double dissociation between motor and visual imagery in the posterior parietal cortex. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Cross-talk between Language Processes and Overt Motor Behavior in the First 200 msec of Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Magdalena Sabaté,et al.  Brain lateralization of motor imagery: motor planning asymmetry as a cause of movement lateralization , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  A Dissociation between the Representation of Tool-use Skills and Hand Dominance: Insights from Left- and Right-handed Callosotomy Patients , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Passingham,et al.  Objects automatically potentiate action: an fMRI study of implicit processing , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  A. Lang,et al.  Apraxia in movement disorders. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Concepts and categories: a cognitive neuropsychological perspective. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[26]  R. Ellis,et al.  Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[27]  G. Gigli,et al.  Effects of Strategies on Mental Rotation Performed by Unilateral Brain Damaged Patients , 2004, Cortex.

[28]  M. Davare,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Dissociating the Role of Ventral and Dorsal Premotor Cortex in Precision Grasping , 2018 .

[29]  K. Heilman,et al.  Two forms of ideomotor apraxia , 1982, Neurology.

[30]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[31]  K M Heilman,et al.  Pantomime comprehension and ideomotor apraxia. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[32]  C D Marsden,et al.  Limb apraxias: higher-order disorders of sensorimotor integration. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  Marco Davare,et al.  Time-course of Posterior Parietal and Occipital Cortex Contribution to Sound Localization , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[35]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  Repetition suppression for performed hand gestures revealed by fMRI , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[36]  Michael Andres,et al.  Hemispheric lateralization of number comparison. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[37]  H. Sakata,et al.  Parietal control of hand action , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Neural substrates of knowledge of hand postures for object grasping and functional object use: Evidence from fMRI , 2006, Brain Research.

[39]  Benoit M. Macq,et al.  Registration and real-time visualization of transcranial magnetic stimulation with 3-D MR images , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[40]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Motor Sequence Complexity and Performing Hand Produce Differential Patterns of Hemispheric Lateralization , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  The organization of conceptual knowledge: the evidence from category-specific semantic deficits , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The different neural correlates of action and functional knowledge in semantic memory: an FMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Action-Related Properties Shape Object Representations in the Ventral Stream , 2007, Neuron.

[44]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Cognitive representations of hand posture in ideomotor apraxia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Raffaella I. Rumiati,et al.  Effects of Strategies on Mental Rotation and Hemispheric Lateralization: Neuropsychological Evidence , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[49]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  The Role of Semantic Memory in Object Use , 1997 .

[50]  H. Sakata,et al.  Deficit of hand preshaping after muscimol injection in monkey parietal cortex , 1994, Neuroreport.

[51]  J. Pellat,et al.  Identification without manipulation: a study of the relations between object use and semantic memory , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Myrna F. Schwartz,et al.  Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: Knowing ‘what for’ but not ‘how’ , 2000 .

[53]  S. Aglioti,et al.  Neural Underpinnings of Gesture Discrimination in Patients with Limb Apraxia , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  H. Poizner,et al.  Progressive Ideomotor Apraxia - Evidence for a Selective Impairment of the Action Production System , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[55]  G. Goldenberg Matching and imitation of hand and finger posturesin patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  The neural basis of tool use. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[57]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  What is the role of motor simulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[58]  G. Orban,et al.  The Representation of Tool Use in Humans and Monkeys: Common and Uniquely Human Features , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  T. Takeda,et al.  Temporal structure of implicit motor imagery in visual hand‐shape discrimination as revealed by MEG , 1998, Neuroreport.