A Dynamic Bayesian Observer Model Reveals Origins of Bias in Visual Path Integration

Path integration is a strategy by which animals track their position by integrating their self-motion velocity. To identify the computational origins of bias in visual path integration, we asked human subjects to navigate in a virtual environment using optic flow and found that they generally traveled beyond the goal location. Such a behavior could stem from leaky integration of unbiased self-motion velocity estimates or from a prior expectation favoring slower speeds that causes velocity underestimation. Testing both alternatives using a probabilistic framework that maximizes expected reward, we found that subjects' biases were better explained by a slow-speed prior than imperfect integration. When subjects integrate paths over long periods, this framework intriguingly predicts a distance-dependent bias reversal due to buildup of uncertainty, which we also confirmed experimentally. These results suggest that visual path integration in noisy environments is limited largely by biases in processing optic flow rather than by leaky integration.

[1]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Model of Interval Timing by Neural Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  Rats and Humans Can Optimally Accumulate Evidence for Decision-Making , 2013, Science.

[3]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Laminar organization of encoding and memory reactivation in the parietal cortex , 2017 .

[4]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Contrast dependency and prior expectations in human speed perception , 2014, Vision Research.

[6]  Krystel R Huxlin,et al.  Spatial integration of optic flow information in direction of heading judgments. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[7]  SIMON BENHAMOU,et al.  Path integration by swimming rats , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  The Effects of Proprioceptive and Visual Feedback on Geographical Orientation in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[9]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Feature expectation heightens visual sensitivity during fine orientation discrimination. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[10]  Michael Jenkin,et al.  Humans can use optic flow to estimate distance of travel , 2001, Vision Research.

[11]  May-Britt Moser,et al.  Path integration in place cells of developing rats , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  M. Carandini,et al.  Testing the Bayesian model of perceived speed , 2002, Vision Research.

[13]  W. Becker,et al.  Estimation of self-turning in the dark: comparison between active and passive rotation , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Inference of perceptual priors from path dynamics of passive self-motion. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  Calibrating Visual Path Integration in VEs , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[17]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in Gerbils and Humans , 1991 .

[18]  Hannah Monyer,et al.  Impaired path integration in mice with disrupted grid cell firing , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  W. Becker,et al.  Perception of angular displacement without landmarks: evidence for Bayesian fusion of vestibular, optokinetic, podokinesthetic, and cognitive information , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Evidence against perfect integration of sensory information during perceptual decision making. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Limited Field of View of Head-Mounted Displays Is Not the Cause of Distance Underestimation in Virtual Environments , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[22]  R. Bogacz Optimal decision-making theories: linking neurobiology with behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  Edvard I. Moser,et al.  Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex , 2015, Nature.

[24]  F. Petzschner,et al.  Iterative Bayesian Estimation as an Explanation for Range and Regression Effects: A Study on Human Path Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  SIMON BENHAMOU,et al.  Path integration in dogs , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  Daniel D. Lee,et al.  Dynamic belief state representations , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[27]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Bias in Human Path Integration Is Predicted by Properties of Grid Cells , 2015, Current Biology.

[28]  Marius Usher,et al.  The Timescale of Perceptual Evidence Integration Can Be Adapted to the Environment , 2013, Current Biology.

[29]  Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al.  Speed tuning in the macaque posterior parietal cortex , 2017, bioRxiv.

[30]  Matthew T. Kaufman,et al.  A category-free neural population supports evolving demands during decision-making , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  Lawrence K. Cormack,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary control of smooth eye movements , 2013, Electronic Imaging.

[32]  A. Etienne Navigation of a Small Mammal by Dead Reckoning and Local Cues , 1992 .

[33]  Matthew Collett,et al.  Path integration in insects , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[34]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Absolute travel distance from optic flow , 2005, Vision Research.

[35]  Lisa M. Giocomo,et al.  Computational Models of Grid Cells , 2011, Neuron.

[36]  Zachary P. Kilpatrick,et al.  Stochastic models of evidence accumulation in changing environments , 2015, bioRxiv.

[37]  W. Becker,et al.  Fusion of vestibular and podokinesthetic information during self-turning towards instructed targets , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  R. Vickerstaff,et al.  Published by The Company of Biologists 2005 doi:10.1242/jeb.01772 Evolving neural models of path integration , 2022 .

[40]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural Activity in Macaque Parietal Cortex Reflects Temporal Integration of Visual Motion Signals during Perceptual Decision Making , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Xaq Pitkow,et al.  Inference in the Brain: Statistics Flowing in Redundant Population Codes , 2017, Neuron.

[42]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[43]  Gerit Pfuhl,et al.  Functional Split between Parietal and Entorhinal Cortices in the Rat , 2012, Neuron.

[44]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Rational Temporal Predictions Can Underlie Apparent Failures to Delay Gratification Theoretical Perspectives on Delay-of-gratification Failure Dual Systems Strength and Depletion Environmental Cuing Hyperbolic Discounting a Normative Perspective Time Prediction during Delay of Gratification Temporal , 2022 .

[45]  Laurence R Harris,et al.  Using optic flow in the far peripheral field. , 2017, Journal of vision.

[46]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Keeping track of the distance from home by leaky integration along veering paths , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Temporal context calibrates interval timing , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Behavioral ecology of odometric memories in desert ants: acquisition, retention, and integration , 2006 .

[50]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[51]  David C. Knill,et al.  Near-optimal spatial integration of optic flow information for direction of heading judgments , 2011 .

[52]  Roozbeh Kiani,et al.  Integration of Direction Cues Is Invariant to the Temporal Gap between Them , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  Kay Thurley,et al.  Magnitude Estimation with Noisy Integrators Linked by an Adaptive Reference , 2016, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[54]  Heiko Hecht,et al.  Locomotor and verbal distance judgments in action and vista space , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  Klaus Pawelzik,et al.  Optimality of Human Contour Integration , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[56]  Johannes Burge,et al.  Continuous psychophysics: Target-tracking to measure visual sensitivity. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[57]  M. Chacron,et al.  Statistics of the Vestibular Input Experienced during Natural Self-Motion: Implications for Neural Processing , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The precision of locomotor odometry in humans , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Travel distance estimation from visual motion by leaky path integration , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Multisensory integration in the estimation of relative path length , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  S. W. Zhang,et al.  Error is proportional to distance measured by honeybees: Weber’s law in the odometer , 1999, Animal Cognition.

[62]  Joseph W Kable,et al.  Normative evidence accumulation in unpredictable environments , 2015, eLife.

[63]  M. Lappe,et al.  Visual estimation of travel distance during walking , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[64]  P. Berkes,et al.  Statistically Optimal Perception and Learning: from Behavior to Neural Representations , 2022 .

[65]  Alan A. Stocker,et al.  Optimal integration of visual speed across different spatiotemporal frequency channels , 2013, NIPS.

[66]  Rudolf Jander,et al.  Short-range homing in the house mouse, Mus musculus: stages in the learning of directions , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[67]  Yoram Burakyy,et al.  Accurate Path Integration in Continuous Attractor Network Models of Grid Cells , 2009 .

[68]  Daniel Mestre,et al.  OSCILLATORY OPTICAL FLOWS IMPROVE THE PERCEPTION OF TRAVELLED DISTANCE IN STATIC OBSERVERS , 2016 .

[69]  Xaq Pitkow,et al.  How the brain might work: statistics flowing in redundant population codes , 2017, 1702.03492.

[70]  Jodie M. Plumert,et al.  Distance perception in real and virtual environments , 2004, APGV '04.