Can infants make transitive inferences?

Researchers have long been interested in the emergence of transitive reasoning abilities (e.g., if A>B and B>C, then A>C). Preschool-aged children are found to make transitive inferences. Additionally, nonhuman animals demonstrate parallel abilities, pointing to evolutionary roots of transitive reasoning. The present research examines whether 16-month-old infants can make transitive inferences about other people's preferences. If an agent prefers object-A over B (A>B) and B over C (B>C), infants seem to reason that she also prefers A over C (A>C) (Experiment 1). Experiment 2 provides indirect evidence that a one-directional linear ordering of the three items (A>B>C) may have helped infants to succeed in the task. These and control results present the first piece of evidence that precursors of transitive reasoning cognitive abilities exist in infancy.

[1]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[2]  J. Edwards Genetic Epistemology , 1971 .

[3]  P. Johnson-Laird The three-term series problem ☆ , 1972 .

[4]  William A. Roberts,et al.  Transitive Inference in Rats: A Test of the Spatial Coding Hypothesis , 1994 .

[5]  Gergely Csibra,et al.  Representation of stable social dominance relations by human infants , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  T. Trabasso,et al.  On the construction and use of representations involving linear order , 1975 .

[7]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  Do 10-month-old infants understand others’ false beliefs? , 2011, Cognition.

[8]  Janellen Huttenlocher,et al.  Constructing spatial images: A strategy in reasoning. , 1968 .

[9]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Comparatives, logical structures, and encoding in a transitive inference task. , 1974 .

[10]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Transitive Inferences and Memory in Young Children , 1971, Nature.

[11]  H. Wellman,et al.  Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. , 1998 .

[12]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  Recognizing the role of perception in action at 6 months. , 2009, Developmental science.

[13]  J. Youniss,et al.  Achievement of inferential transitivity and its relation to serial ordering. , 1968, Child development.

[14]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Can a Self-Propelled Box Have a Goal? , 2005, Psychological science.

[15]  J. Smedslund Development of concrete transitivity of length in children. , 1963, Child development.

[16]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? , 2007, Cognition.

[17]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Social complexity predicts transitive reasoning in prosimian primates , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  A. Tversky Intransitivity of preferences. , 1969 .

[19]  G. Berntson,et al.  Processing of ordinality and transitivity by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.

[20]  E. Spelke,et al.  The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research in social cognition , 1981 .

[21]  E. Spelke Initial knowledge: six suggestions , 1994, Cognition.

[22]  L. Breslow Reevaluation of the Literature on the Development of Transitive Inferences. , 1981 .

[23]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning , 2002, Cognition.

[24]  M. Braine DEVELOPMENT OF A GRASP OF TRANSITIVITY OF LENGTH: A REPLY TO SMEDSLUND. , 1964, Child development.

[25]  L. Acredolo,et al.  Developmental changes in the effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior. , 1980 .

[26]  B. McGonigle,et al.  Are monkeys logical? , 1977, Nature.

[27]  C. Riley The representation of comparative relations and the transitive inference task , 1976 .

[28]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. , 1969 .

[29]  D. Gillan Reasoning in the chimpanzee: II. Transitive inference. , 1981 .

[30]  Herbert S. Terrace,et al.  Memory and Representation of Serial Order by Children, Monkeys, and Pigeons , 1994 .

[31]  J. Dana,et al.  Transitivity of preferences. , 2011, Psychological review.

[32]  P. Harris,et al.  Transitive inferences by 4-year-old children? , 1975 .

[33]  Christian J. Rapold,et al.  Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in Hominid spatial cognition , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  J. Piaget,et al.  Child's Conception Of Geometry , 1960 .

[35]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[36]  J. Staddon,et al.  Transitive inference formation in pigeons. , 1991 .

[37]  S. Handel,et al.  Social reasoning and spatial paralogic. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  Three-month-old infants attribute goals to a non-human agent. , 2011, Developmental science.

[39]  B. Sodian,et al.  Now I see it but you don't: 14-month-olds can represent another person's visual perspective. , 2007, Developmental science.

[40]  Guillermo Paz-y-Miño C,et al.  Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance , 2004, Nature.

[41]  K. O’regan,et al.  What Children Do in Spite of Adults' Hypotheses , 1973, Nature.

[42]  C. Collyer,et al.  The development of transitive inference: a review of recent approaches. , 1978, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  Do you see what I see? Infants' reasoning about others' incomplete perceptions. , 2010, Developmental science.