Experience- and Age-Related Outgrowth of Intrinsic Neurons in the Mushroom Bodies of the Adult Worker Honeybee

A worker honeybee performs tasks within the hive for approximately the first 3 weeks of adult life. After this time, it becomes a forager, flying repeatedly to collect food outside of the hive for the remainder of its 5–6 week life. Previous studies have shown that foragers have an increased volume of neuropil associated with the mushroom bodies, a brain region involved in learning, memory, and sensory integration. We report here that growth of the mushroom body neuropil in adult bees occurs throughout adult life and continues after bees begin to forage. Studies using Golgi impregnation asked whether the growth of the collar region of the mushroom body neuropil was a result of growth of the dendritic processes of the mushroom body intrinsic neurons, the Kenyon cells. Branching and length of dendrites in the collar region of the calyces were strongly correlated with worker age, but when age-matched bees were directly compared, those with foraging experience had longer, more branched dendrites than bees that had foraged less or not at all. The density of Kenyon cell dendritic spines remained constant regardless of age or behavioral state. Older and more experienced foragers therefore have a greater total number of dendritic spines in the mushroom body neuropil. Our findings indicate that, under natural conditions, the cytoarchitectural complexity of neurons in the mushroom bodies of adult honeybees increases as a function of increasing age, but that foraging experience promotes additional dendritic branching and growth.

[1]  Richard G. Coss,et al.  Changes in morphology of dendritic spines on honeybee calycal interneurons associated with cumulative nursing and foraging experiences , 1980, Brain Research.

[2]  J. Young Structure and function in the nervous systems of invertebrates , 1966 .

[3]  S. Schäfer,et al.  Proliferation and programmed cell death of neuronal precursors in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee , 2000, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[4]  H. Gundersen,et al.  Efficient estimation of cell volume and number using the nucleator and the disector , 1990, Journal of microscopy.

[5]  M. Koenderink,et al.  Postnatal maturation of the layer III pyramidal neurons in the human prefrontal cortex: a quantitative Golgi analysis , 1994, Brain Research.

[6]  J. Larson,et al.  Effects of unilateral and bilateral training in a reaching task on dendritic branching of neurons in the rat motor-sensory forelimb cortex. , 1985, Behavioral and neural biology.

[7]  P. Mobbs The Brain of the Honeybee Apis Mellifera. I. The Connections and Spatial Organization of the Mushroom Bodies , 1982 .

[8]  Mark J. West,et al.  New stereological methods for counting neurons , 1993, Neurobiology of Aging.

[9]  G. Robinson,et al.  Selective neuroanatomical plasticity and division of labour in the honeybee , 1993, Nature.

[10]  G. Robinson Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. , 1992, Annual review of entomology.

[11]  Gene E. Robinson,et al.  Hormonal regulation of behavioural development in the honey bee is based on changes in the rate of juvenile hormone biosynthesis , 1991 .

[12]  A. S. Edwards,et al.  Ontogeny of orientation flight in the honeybee revealed by harmonic radar , 2000, Nature.

[13]  H. J. G. GUNDERSEN,et al.  Some new, simple and efficient stereological methods and their use in pathological research and diagnosis , 1988, APMIS : acta pathologica, microbiologica, et immunologica Scandinavica.

[14]  S. Tieman,et al.  Development of the dendritic fields of layer 3 pyramidal cells in the kitten's visual cortex , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[15]  G. Robinson,et al.  Hormonal and Genetic Control of Behavioral Integration in Honey Bee Colonies , 1989, Science.

[16]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Morphology and sensory modality of mushroom body extrinsic neurons in the brain of the cockroach, Periplaneta americana , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[17]  D. N. Spinelli,et al.  Early experience effect on dendritic branching in normally reared kittens , 1980, Experimental Neurology.

[18]  Sholl Da Dendritic organization in the neurons of the visual and motor cortices of the cat. , 1953 .

[19]  L M Cruz-Orive,et al.  Application of the Cavalieri principle and vertical sections method to lung: estimation of volume and pleural surface area , 1988, Journal of microscopy.

[20]  E. Uemura,et al.  Age-related changes in prefrontal cortex of Macaca mulatta: Quantitative analysis of dendritic branching patterns , 1980, Experimental Neurology.

[21]  E. Bennett,et al.  Effects of environmental enrichment and impoverishment on rat cerebral cortex. , 1972, Journal of neurobiology.

[22]  G. Robinson,et al.  Volume Changes in the Mushroom Bodies of Adult Honey Bee Queens , 1995, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[23]  H. Klüver,et al.  A method for the combined staining of cells and fibers in the nervous system. , 1953, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[24]  G. Robinson,et al.  Regulation of honey bee division of labor by colony age demography , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[25]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Structural plasticity in the Drosophila brain , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  H. Uylings,et al.  Environmental influences on the neocortex in later life. , 1978, Progress in brain research.

[27]  Richard G. Coss,et al.  Rapid dendritic spine stem shortening during one-trial learning: The honeybee's first orientation flight , 1982, Brain Research.

[28]  J. Juraska The development of pyramidal neurons after eye opening in the visual cortex of hooded rats: A quantitative study , 1982, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  G. Robinson,et al.  Larval and pupal development of the mushroom bodies in the honey bee, Apis mellifera , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Rearing Complexity Affects Branching of Dendrites in the Visual Cortex of the Rat , 1972, Science.

[31]  B. Kolb,et al.  Age, Experience and the Changing Brain , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[32]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[33]  G. Robinson,et al.  Experience-expectant plasticity in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[34]  E. Bennett,et al.  Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  G. Robinson,et al.  Behavioral development in the honey bee: toward the study of learning under natural conditions. , 1995, Learning & memory.

[36]  G. Robinson,et al.  Honeybee colony integration: worker-worker interactions mediate hormonally regulated plasticity in division of labor. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  L. Bertholf The Moults of the Honeybee , 1925 .

[38]  H J Gundersen,et al.  The efficiency of systematic sampling in stereology and its prediction * , 1987, Journal of microscopy.

[39]  M. Burrows The Neurobiology of an Insect Brain , 1996 .

[40]  S. B. Kater,et al.  Dendritic spines: cellular specializations imparting both stability and flexibility to synaptic function. , 1994, Annual review of neuroscience.

[41]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Pattern of dendritic branching in occipital cortex of rats reared in complex environments. , 1973, Experimental neurology.

[42]  R. Menzel,et al.  Development and experience lead to increased volume of subcompartments of the honeybee mushroom body. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[43]  T. Robinson,et al.  Brain Plasticity and Behavior , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[44]  G. Robinson,et al.  Effects of experience and juvenile hormone on the organization of the mushroom bodies of honey bees. , 1995, Journal of neurobiology.

[45]  Robert E. Page,et al.  Genetic determination of guarding and undertaking in honey-bee colonies , 1988, Nature.