A Causal-Power Theory of Focal Sets
暂无分享,去创建一个
Jooyong Park | Patricia W. Cheng | Keith J. Holyoak | Aaron S. Yarlas | K. Holyoak | P. Cheng | A. Yarlas | Jooyong Park
[1] J. Pearce,et al. Effects of extinction with a compound conditioned stimulus , 1991 .
[2] H. M. Jenkins,et al. JUDGMENT OF CONTINGENCY BETWEEN RESPONSES AND OUTCOMES. , 1965, Psychological monographs.
[3] A. Premack,et al. Causal cognition : a multidisciplinary debate , 1996 .
[4] P. Cheng. From covariation to causation: A causal power theory. , 1997 .
[5] A. Michotte. The perception of causality , 1963 .
[6] T. Shultz. Rules of Causal Attribution. , 1982 .
[7] W. F. Prokasy,et al. Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .
[8] R. Rescorla,et al. Extinction of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[9] M. Best,et al. Extinguishing Conditioned Inhibition in Flavour-Aversion Learning: Effects of Repeated Testing and Extinction of the Excitatory Element , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[10] R. Rescorla. Conditioned inhibition of fear resulting from negative CS-US contingencies. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[11] E. Wasserman,et al. Cue Competition in Causality Judgments: The Role of Nonpresentation of Compound Stimulus Elements , 1994 .
[12] R. R. Miller,et al. The comparator hypothesis of conditioned response generation: manifest conditioned excitation and inhibition as a function of relative excitatory strengths of CS and conditioning context at the time of testing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[13] Yunnwen Lien,et al. The role of coherence in differentiating genuine from spurious causes , 1996 .
[14] G. Bower,et al. From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988 .
[15] H. Roitblat,et al. Comparative approaches to cognitive science , 1995 .
[16] R. Rescorla,et al. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .
[17] K. Holyoak,et al. Predictive and diagnostic learning within causal models: asymmetries in cue competition. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[18] J. Pearce. Similarity and Discrimination: A Selective Review and a Connectionist Model , 1994 .
[19] H. Fowler,et al. Inhibition as a "slave" process: deactivation of conditioned inhibition through extinction of conditioned excitation. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[20] W. Friedman. The Developmental psychology of time , 1982 .
[21] R. Rescorla. Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[22] John R. Anderson,et al. Causal inferences as perceptual judgments , 1995, Memory & cognition.
[23] H. Kelley. Attribution theory in social psychology , 1967 .
[24] P. Wiener,et al. The Encyclopedia of Philosophy. , 1968 .
[25] R. Shillcock,et al. Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1995 .
[26] C. Gallistel. The organization of learning , 1990 .
[27] David Hume. A Treatise of Human Nature: Being an Attempt to introduce the experimental Method of Reasoning into Moral Subjects , 1972 .
[28] David R. Shanks,et al. CATEGORIZATION BY A CONNECTIONIST NETWORK , 1991 .
[29] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Assessment of the Rescorla-Wagner model. , 1995 .
[30] D. Medin,et al. The role of covariation versus mechanism information in causal attribution , 1995, Cognition.
[31] P. Cheng,et al. Covariation in natural causal induction. , 1992 .
[32] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Excitation and inhibition as a function of posttraining extinction of the excitatory cue used in pavlovian inhibition training , 1990 .
[33] Laura R. Novick,et al. A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction. , 1990 .
[35] E. Madden,et al. Causal powers: A theory of natural necessity , 1975 .
[36] P. Balsam,et al. Context and Learning , 1985 .
[37] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Information processing in animals : conditioned inhibition , 1985 .
[38] P. Cheng,et al. Causes versus enabling conditions , 1991, Cognition.
[39] Wesley C. Salmon,et al. Statistical explanation & statistical relevance , 1971 .
[40] David R. Shanks,et al. Associative versus contingency accounts of category learning: Reply to Melz, Cheng, Holyoak, and Waldmann (1993). , 1993 .
[41] P. White,et al. A theory of causal processing , 1989 .
[42] Patricia W. Cheng,et al. Cue Competition in Human Categorization: Contingency or the Rescorla-Wagner Learning Rule? Comment on Shanks (1991) , 1993 .
[43] N. Cartwright. Nature's Capacities and Their Measurement , 1995 .
[44] Patricia W. Cheng,et al. Separating Causal Laws from Casual Facts: Pressing the Limits of Statistical Relevance , 1993 .
[45] E. Kremer. The Rescorla-Wagner model: losses in associative strength in compound conditioned stimuli. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[46] W. Ahn,et al. Causal Attribution as a Search for Underlying Mechanisms: An Explanation of the Conjunction Fallacy and the Discounting Principle , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.
[47] Knowledge-based assumptions in causal attribution , 1992 .