Beaconing Signalization Substantially Reduces Blind Pedestrians’ Veer on Snow-Covered Pavement

Veering outside of crosswalks is a common problem experienced by individuals who are blind. One technology found to be effective for reducing this veer when other guidance cues are absent is audible beaconing. However, veering in general and veering from crosswalks in particular have been studied primarily on smooth, flat walking surfaces such as clear pavement. This experiment compared veering on clear pavement with veering on snow-covered pavement, with and without audible beaconing. Eleven blind participants traveling with long canes attempted to walk a straight path for 72 ft (21.9 m), a typical length of a six-lane crosswalk. Beaconing substantially reduced veering at 36 ft (11.0 m) and 72 ft from the starting point and enabled participants to remain within a simulated crosswalk. Walking on snow was not found to affect veering but did increase the number of steps taken. The findings suggest that in snowy and clear conditions alike, audible beaconing is an effective wayfinding tool for intersections equipped with accessible pedestrian signals.

[1]  Janet M Barlow,et al.  Far-Side Audible Beaconing of Accessible Pedestrian Signals , 2014 .

[2]  Billie Louise Bentzen,et al.  Directional guidance from audible pedestrian signals for street crossing , 2004, Ergonomics.

[3]  Chen-Fu Liao Using a Smartphone Application to Support Visually Impaired Pedestrians at Signalized Intersection Crossings , 2013 .

[4]  Gordon E Legge,et al.  Variability in stepping direction explains the veering behavior of blind walkers. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  D. Kim,et al.  Travel in Adverse Winter Weather Conditions by Blind Pedestrians: Effect of Cane Tip Design on Travel on Snow , 2015, Journal of visual impairment & blindness.

[6]  A. Uematsu,et al.  Preferred step frequency minimizes veering during natural human walking , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  D.A. Guth,et al.  Tests of Blind Pedestrians’ Use of Traffic Sounds for Street-crossing Alignment , 1989 .

[8]  Dona Sauerburger Street Crossings: Analyzing Risks, Developing Strategies, and Making Decisions , 2005 .

[9]  Janet M Barlow,et al.  Effectiveness of Audible and Tactile Heading Cues at Complex Intersections for Pedestrians who are Blind , 2013 .

[10]  Janet M Barlow,et al.  Blind Pedestrians and the Changing Technology and Geometry of Signalized Intersections: Safety, Orientation, and Independence , 2005, Journal of visual impairment & blindness.

[11]  Torben Poulsen Acoustic traffic signal for blind pedestrians , 1982 .

[12]  Ingvar Holmér,et al.  Gait muscle activity during walking on an inclined icy surface. , 2008, Industrial health.

[13]  M. Ernst,et al.  Walking Straight into Circles , 2009, Current Biology.

[14]  D. Guth Space saving statistics: An introduction to constant error, variable error, and absolute error. , 1990 .

[15]  J. Lundälv The Experiences of Visually Impaired People in Sweden as Pedestrians and Cyclists , 2001 .

[16]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  GPOWER: A general power analysis program , 1996 .

[17]  R. Cham,et al.  Lower extremity corrective reactions to slip events. , 2001, Journal of biomechanics.

[18]  Janet M Barlow,et al.  Walking between the Lines: Nonvisual Cues for Maintaining Headings during Street Crossings , 2011 .

[19]  Daniel H. Ashmead,et al.  Auditory Perception of Motor Vehicle Travel Paths , 2012, Hum. Factors.

[20]  D. Guth,et al.  Veering by Blind Pedestrians: Individual Differences and Their Implications for Instruction , 1995 .