Knowing Where Things Are in the Second Year of Life: Implications for Hippocampal Development

Prior data have revealed striking contrasts between 18 and 24-month-old children in place learning, an ability knownto dependon the hippo campus(Newcombe, Huttenlocher, Drummey, & Wiley, 1998). The current research examined the development of three other basic abilities of mature spatial competence: the representation of multiple locations, the learning of relations among objects, and the recall of a single location after a substantial filled delay. Results indicated a transition from 18 to 24 months in all three abilities. This evidence supports a general transition in spatial representation that occurs towards the end of infancy. Existing neurobehavioral data suggest that a corresponding change in hippocampal functioning underlies this development.

[1]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.

[2]  J. Bachevalier,et al.  A comparison of children's performance on two recognition memory tasks: delayed nonmatch-to-sample versus visual paired-comparison. , 1993, Developmental psychobiology.

[3]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.

[4]  M. Mishkin,et al.  A selective mnemonic role for the hippocampus in monkeys: memory for the location of objects , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  J. B. Ranck,et al.  Spatial firing patterns of hippocampal complex-spike cells in a fixed environment , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  R P Kesner,et al.  Effects of hippocampal and parietal cortex lesions on memory for egocentric distance and spatial location information in rats. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  L. Nadel,et al.  Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[9]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Hippocampectomized monkeys can remember one place but not two , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  R P Kesner,et al.  Short-term memory for duration and distance in humans: role of the hippocampus. , 2001, Neuropsychology.

[11]  A. Churchland,et al.  Early developments in the ability to understand the relation between stimulus and reward. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[12]  J. Deloache,et al.  Very young children's memory for the location of objects in a large-scale environment. , 1983, Child development.

[13]  J. Bachevalier,et al.  The Hippocampal/Parahippocampal Regions and Recognition Memory: Insights from Visual Paired Comparison versus Object-Delayed Nonmatching in Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Learning to find your way: a role for the human hippocampal formation , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Single unit activity in the rat hippocampus during a spatial memory task , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  Comparative effects of excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampus and septum/diagonal band on conditional visual discrimination and spatial learning , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  J. Fagan The Paired‐Comparison Paradigm and Infant Intelligence , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  O. Pascalis,et al.  Neonatal aspiration lesions of the hippocampal formation impair visual recognition memory when assessed by paired‐comparison task but not by delayed nonmatching‐to‐sample task , 1999, Hippocampus.

[19]  E. Spelke,et al.  Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.

[20]  P. Bauer,et al.  When the event is more than the sum of its parts: 9-month-olds' long-term ordered recall. , 1999, Memory.

[21]  J. Reznick,et al.  Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[22]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  THE DEVELOPMENT OF SPATIAL LOCATION CODING: PLACE LEARNING AND DEAD RECKONING IN THE SECOND AND THIRD YEARS , 1998 .

[23]  W. Overman Performance on Traditional Matching to Sample, Non‐Matching to Sample, and Object Discrimination Tasks by 12‐ to 32‐Month‐Old Children , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  Young children's performance on a task sensitive to the memory functions of the medial temporal lobe in adults--the delayed nonmatching-to-sample task--reveals problems that are due to non-memory-related task demands. , 1994 .

[25]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Infants' coding of location in continuous space. , 1999 .

[26]  A. Diamond,et al.  Young children's performance on a task sensitive to the memory functions of the medial temporal lobe in adults--the delayed nonmatching-to-sample task--reveals problems that are due to non-memory-related task demands. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  A. Diamond,et al.  Early success in using the relation between stimuli and rewards to deduce an abstract rule: perceived physical connection is key. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[28]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Infantile Amnesia: A Neurobiological Perspective , 1984 .

[30]  Shelley Channon,et al.  T-Maze Discrimination and Reversal Learning After Unilateral Temporal or Frontal Lobe Lesions in Man , 1991, Cortex.

[31]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Conditional spatial discrimination in humans with hypoxic brain injury , 2000, Psychobiology.

[32]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.

[33]  K. Nakamura,et al.  Monkey hippocampal neurons related to spatial and nonspatial functions. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Inah Lee,et al.  Differential roles of dorsal hippocampal subregions in spatial working memory with short versus intermediate delay. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  L. Jarrard On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[36]  J. Aggleton,et al.  A comparison of egocentric and allocentric spatial memory in a patient with selective hippocampal damage , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[38]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[39]  A. Diamond,et al.  Rate of Maturation of the Hippocampus and the Developmental Progression of Children's Performance on the Delayed Non‐Matching to Sample and Visual Paired Comparison Tasks a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[40]  E. Bushnell,et al.  The spatial coding strategies of one-year-old infants in a locomotor search task. , 1995, Child development.

[41]  D. Kimble The effects of bilateral hippocampal lesions in rats. , 1963, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[42]  J M Mandler,et al.  Long-term recall of event sequences in infancy. , 1995, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Recalling Routes around London: Activation of the Right Hippocampus in Taxi Drivers , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  The Coding of Spatial Location in Young Children , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.