Localization of shapes: eye movements and perception compared
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Eileen Kowler,et al. Saccadic localization of occluded targets , 2000, Vision Research.
[2] Erik Blaser,et al. The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets , 1995, Vision Research.
[3] H. Collewijn,et al. Differences in accuracy of human saccades between stationary and jumping targets , 1989, Vision Research.
[4] D. Proffitt,et al. The roles of contour and luminance distribution in determining perceived centers within shapes , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[5] John M. Findlay,et al. The spatial signal for saccadic eye movements emphasizes visual boundaries , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[6] D. Whitaker,et al. Centroid evaluation in the vernier alignment of random dot clusters , 1988, Vision Research.
[7] L. Kaufman,et al. Spontaneous fixation tendencies for visual forms , 1969 .
[8] D. Levi,et al. Localization of a Peripheral Patch: The Role of Blur and Spatial Frequency , 1996, Vision Research.
[9] Eileen Kowler,et al. The role of location probability in the programming of saccades: Implications for “center-of-gravity” tendencies , 1989, Vision Research.
[10] A. A. Skavenski,et al. Miniature eye movement. , 1973, Science.
[11] Suzanne P. McKee,et al. Integration regions for visual hyperacuity , 1977, Vision Research.
[12] P. Glimcher,et al. Representation of averaging saccades in the superior colliculus of the monkey , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] J. Cronly-Dillon,et al. Vision and visual dysfunction. , 1994, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.
[14] Eileen Kowler,et al. Shapes, surfaces and saccades , 1999, Vision Research.
[15] Eileen Kowler,et al. The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—II. Single target displacements , 1979, Vision Research.
[16] D. Levi,et al. Spatial localization without visual references , 1992, Vision Research.
[17] E Kowler,et al. Saccadic localization of eccentric forms. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[18] Ronald M. Hansen,et al. Spatial localization during pursuit eye movements , 1979, Vision Research.
[19] Dennis M. Levi,et al. Collinearity improves alignment Response to Keeble and Hess (1998) , 2002, Vision Research.
[20] Christian Coëffé,et al. Reducing the influence of non-target stimuli on saccade accuracy: Predictability and latency effects , 1987, Vision Research.
[21] H. Collewijn,et al. The function of visual search and memory in sequential looking tasks , 1995, Vision Research.
[22] F. Ottes,et al. Latency dependence of colour-based target vs nontarget discrimination by the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.
[23] Eileen Kowler,et al. Saccadic localization of random dot targets , 1998, Vision Research.
[24] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[25] D M Levi,et al. Selective Attention to Specific Location Cues: The Peak and Center of a Patch are Equally Accessible as Location Cues , 1998, Perception.
[26] Eileen Kowler. Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.
[27] Brian J. Murphy,et al. Simple forms and fluctuations of the line of sight: Implications for motor theories of form processing , 1974 .
[28] J. O'regan,et al. Eye fixations near corners: evidence for a centre of gravity calculation based on contrast, rather than luminance or curvature , 1994, Vision Research.
[29] R. Carpenter,et al. Movements of the Eyes , 1978 .
[30] M. Morgan,et al. Biases and sensitivities in geometrical illusions , 1990, Vision Research.
[31] R. Steinman. Effect of Target Size, Luminance, and Color on Monocular Fixation* , 1965 .
[32] J. Findlay. Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.
[33] Eileen Kowler,et al. The control of saccadic adaptation: implications for the scanning of natural visual scenes , 2000, Vision Research.
[34] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. The cortical organization of visual perception and visuomotor control , 1995 .
[35] H Collewijn,et al. Binocular eye movements and the perception of depth. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.
[36] H. D. Crane,et al. Accurate three-dimensional eyetracker. , 1978, Applied optics.
[37] Eric Mjolsness,et al. A center-of-mass computation describes the precision of random dot displacement discrimination , 1992, Vision Research.
[38] L. Kaufman,et al. “Center-of-gravity” Tendencies for fixations and flow patterns , 1969 .
[39] DAVID WHITAKER,et al. Centroid Analysis Predicts Visual Localization of First- and Second-order Stimuli , 1996, Vision Research.
[40] Milan Vego,et al. Center of Gravity , 2000 .
[41] D. R. T. Keeble,et al. PII: S0042-6989(97)00234-4 , 2003 .
[42] Carolina M. Zingale,et al. Planning sequences of saccades , 1987, Vision Research.
[43] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Embodiment is the foundation, not a level , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[44] David Whitaker,et al. The effect of suprathreshold contrast on stimulus centroid and its implications for the perceived location of objects , 1998, Vision Research.
[45] J. V. Gisbergen,et al. Collicular ensemble coding of saccades based on vector summation , 1987, Neuroscience.
[46] S. Coren,et al. Effect of Non-Target Stimuli upon Length of Voluntary Saccades , 1972, Perceptual and motor skills.
[47] Dennis M. Levi,et al. Collinearity improves alignment. Authors' reply , 2002 .
[48] N BOCHEVA,et al. Different Mechanisms may Underlie the Performance in Relative Localization Tasks , 1996, Vision Research.
[49] D. Sparks,et al. Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.
[50] Paul V. McGraw,et al. Alignment of separated patches: multiple location tags , 1999, Vision Research.
[51] B. Dosher,et al. The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.