Psychopathic traits, victim distress and aggression in children.

BACKGROUND The relationship between psychopathic traits and aggression in children may be explained by their reduced sensitivity to signs of distress in others. Emotional cues such as fear and sadness function to make the perpetrator aware of the victim's distress and supposedly inhibit aggression. As children high in psychopathic traits show a reduced sensitivity to others' distress, these important interpersonal signals cannot perform their aggression inhibiting function. The present experiment tested the hypothesis that aggression in children with psychopathic traits can be attenuated by making distress cues more salient. METHODS N = 224 participants from the community (53 % boys, M age = 10.81 years, SD = 0.92) played a computer-based competitive reaction-time game against a simulated opponent by blasting him or her with loud noise through a headphone. The salience of the opponent's distress was increased for half of the participants (randomly selected) by a written message expressing his or her fear. Psychopathic traits were assessed using the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory - Child Version (Van Baardewijk et al., 2008). RESULTS As expected, regression analysis showed that psychopathic traits were strongly related to aggression in the no distress condition but not in the distress condition. Thus, the relation between psychopathic traits and aggression depended upon the salience of the opponent's distress. CONCLUSIONS It was concluded that children with psychopathic traits are indeed prone to act aggressively, but also that this aggression is dynamic and is dependent upon circumstances. Their aggression can be attenuated by a salient display of others' distress. These results suggest that empathy based treatment techniques may reduce aggression in children with psychopathic traits.

[1]  B. Bushman,et al.  Trumping shame by blasts of noise: narcissism, self-esteem, shame, and aggression in young adolescents. , 2008, Child development.

[2]  S. Thomaes,et al.  Measuring psychopathic traits in children through self-report. The development of the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory-Child Version. , 2008, International journal of law and psychiatry.

[3]  M. Willoughby,et al.  Effects of Methylphenidate and Behavior Modification on the Social and Academic Behavior of Children With Disruptive Behavior Disorders: The Moderating Role of Callous/Unemotional Traits , 2007, Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology : the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53.

[4]  M. Dadds,et al.  Stability and Malleability of Callous-Unemotional Traits During Treatment for Childhood Conduct Problems , 2007, Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology : the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53.

[5]  Daniel C. Murrie,et al.  Youth with psychopathy features are not a discrete class: a taxometric analysis. , 2007, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[6]  B. Bushman,et al.  I wish I were a warrior: the role of wishful identification in the effects of violent video games on aggression in adolescent boys. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[7]  H. Andershed,et al.  Convergent Validity of the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI) , 2007, Assessment.

[8]  B. Bettencourt,et al.  Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: a meta-analytic review. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  P. Lichtenstein,et al.  A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. , 2006, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  P. Greenbaum,et al.  Construct Validity of the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI) and the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) With Justice-Involved Adolescents , 2006 .

[11]  P. Frick,et al.  The association of psychopathic traits with aggression and delinquency in non-referred boys and girls. , 2005, Behavioral sciences & the law.

[12]  M. Dadds,et al.  The treatment of conduct problems in children with callous-unemotional traits. , 2005, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[13]  Aaron D. J. Frost,et al.  Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: a community study. , 2005, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[14]  S. Scott,et al.  Deafness to fear in boys with psychopathic tendencies. , 2005, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[15]  Alan M. Gross,et al.  Childhood bullying: Current empirical findings and future directions for research , 2004 .

[16]  Jennifer L. Skeem,et al.  Views of the downward extension: comparing the Youth Version of the Psychopathy Checklist with the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory. , 2003, Behavioral sciences & the law.

[17]  Christopher T. Barry,et al.  Callous-Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems in the Prediction of Conduct Problem Severity, Aggression, and Self-Report of Delinquency , 2003, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[18]  Lori Ann Vogt Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for caring and Justice. , 2003 .

[19]  W. Pelham,et al.  Reactive Aggression in Boys with Disruptive Behavior Disorders: Behavior, Physiology, and Affect , 2002, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[20]  C. Izard,et al.  Emotion processes in normal and abnormal development and preventive intervention , 2002, Development and Psychopathology.

[21]  R. Blair,et al.  A Selective Impairment in the Processing of Sad and Fearful Expressions in Children with Psychopathic Tendencies , 2001, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[22]  R. Blair,et al.  Recognition of Emotion in Facial Expressions and Vocal Tones in Children With Psychopathic Tendencies , 2001, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[23]  M. Underwood,et al.  Children's responses to same- and other-gender peers: an experimental investigation with 8-, 10-, and 12-year-olds. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[24]  R. Blair,et al.  Expression recognition and behavioural problems in early adolescence , 2000 .

[25]  R. Blair,et al.  Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies , 1999 .

[26]  J. Zeman,et al.  Social-contextual influences on expectancies for managing anger and sadness: the transition from middle childhood to adolescence. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[27]  R. Blair Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies , 1997 .

[28]  C. Anderson,et al.  External Validity of “Trivial” Experiments: The Case of Laboratory Aggression , 1997 .

[29]  Clara E. Hill,et al.  The current state of empathy research , 1996 .

[30]  J. Garber,et al.  Display rules for anger, sadness, and pain: it depends on who is watching. , 1996, Child development.

[31]  P. Frick,et al.  Current perspectives on conduct disorder , 1996, Current psychiatry reports.

[32]  R. O. Friedel Early sea changes in borderline personality disorder , 1996, Current psychiatry reports.

[33]  R. Blair,et al.  A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath , 1995, Cognition.

[34]  P. Frick,et al.  Psychopathy and conduct problems in children. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[35]  W. Pelham,et al.  Aggression in boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Methylphenidate effects on naturalistically observed aggression, response to provocation, and social information processing , 1992, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[36]  Norman Miller,et al.  Evidence for a General Construct of Aggression , 1989 .

[37]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  R. Loeber,et al.  The stability of antisocial and delinquent child behavior: a review. , 1982, Child development.

[39]  S. Weisberg,et al.  Residuals and Influence in Regression , 1982 .

[40]  H. Solomon,et al.  The Treatment , 1976, Corporate Media Production.

[41]  S. Taylor Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and the tendency to inhibit aggression. , 1967, Journal of personality.

[42]  G. Harris,et al.  Treatment of Psychopathy: A Review of Empirical Findings. , 2006 .

[43]  M. Hoffman,et al.  Empathy and moral development , 2000 .

[44]  W. Koops,et al.  Van lastig tot misdadig , 1998 .

[45]  A. Zeichner,et al.  Construct validity of a competitive reaction‐time aggression paradigm , 1995 .

[46]  Larry E. Toothaker,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions , 1991 .

[47]  John M. Gottman,et al.  Social and emotional development in a relational context: Friendship interaction from early childhood to adolescence. , 1989 .

[48]  G. Ladd,et al.  Peer relationships in child development. , 1989 .

[49]  S. Modgil,et al.  The cognitive-developmental approach to morality , 1976 .

[50]  L. Kohlberg A Cognitive-Developmental Approach to Moral Education. , 1972 .