Drawing sounds: representing tones and chords spatially

Research on the crossmodal correspondences has revealed that seemingly unrelated perceptual information can be matched across the senses in a manner that is consistent across individuals. An interesting extension of this line of research is to study how sensory information biases action. In the present study, we investigated whether different sounds (i.e. tones and piano chords) would bias participants’ hand movements in a free movement task. Right-handed participants were instructed to move a computer mouse in order to represent three tones and two chords. They also had to rate each sound in terms of three visual analogue scales (slow–fast, unpleasant–pleasant, and weak–strong). The results demonstrate that tones and chords influence hand movements, with higher-(lower-)pitched sounds giving rise to a significant bias towards upper (lower) locations in space. These results are discussed in terms of the literature on forward models, embodied cognition, crossmodal correspondences, and mental imagery. Potential applications sports and rehabilitation are discussed briefly.

[1]  C. Keysers,et al.  The Observation and Execution of Actions Share Motor and Somatosensory Voxels in all Tested Subjects: Single-Subject Analyses of Unsmoothed fMRI Data , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  Dominic McIver Lopes,et al.  Hearing and Seeing Musical Expression , 2009 .

[4]  Donald W. Pfaff,et al.  Neuroscience in the 21st Century: From Basic to Clinical , 2013 .

[5]  Leo Treitler,et al.  The Early History of Music Writing in the West , 1982 .

[6]  F. Marmolejo-Ramos,et al.  Cross-modal metaphorical mapping of spoken emotion words onto vertical space , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[7]  Francesco Pavani,et al.  Acoustical Vision of Neglected Stimuli: Interaction among Spatially Converging Audiovisual Inputs in Neglect Patients , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  C. Spence,et al.  The sweetest thing: the influence of angularity, symmetry, and the number of elements on shape-valence and shape-taste matches , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[9]  Yuki Yamada,et al.  Emotion biases voluntary vertical action only with visible cues. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[10]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Why a Big Mac Is a Good Mac: Associations between Affect and Size , 2008 .

[11]  C. Spence Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[12]  C. Spence,et al.  INTERACTION OF SIGHT AND SOUND IN THE PERCEPTION AND EXPERIENCE OF MUSICAL PERFORMANCE , 2016 .

[13]  Ana Tajadura-Jiménez,et al.  Emotion-inducing approaching sounds shape the boundaries of multisensory peripersonal space , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Y. Mauss,et al.  Blindness and brain plasticity: contribution of mental imagery? An fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  Robert D. Melara,et al.  Interaction between synesthetically corresponding dimensions. , 1987 .

[16]  Henry E. Heffner,et al.  Evolution of Sound Localization in Mammals , 1992 .

[17]  Stephen R. Arnott,et al.  The auditory dorsal pathway: Orienting vision , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[18]  Stefan Van der Stigchel,et al.  Multisensory Stimulation to Improve Low- and Higher-Level Sensory Deficits after Stroke: A Systematic Review , 2015, Neuropsychology Review.

[19]  Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos,et al.  A simple method for the detection of outliers , 2009 .

[20]  L. Trainor,et al.  Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding of rhythmic movement , 2007, Cognition.

[21]  C. C. Pratt The spatial character of high and low tones. , 1930 .

[22]  David W. Franklin,et al.  Computational Mechanisms of Sensorimotor Control , 2011, Neuron.

[23]  M. Kiyosawa,et al.  Auditory Triggered Mental Imagery of Shape Involves Visual Association Areas in Early Blind Humans , 2001, NeuroImage.

[24]  Peter Wolf,et al.  Sonification and haptic feedback in addition to visual feedback enhances complex motor task learning , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  K. Scherer,et al.  Cue utilization in emotion attribution from auditory stimuli , 1977 .

[26]  Laurence R Harris,et al.  Which Direction Is up for a High Pitch? , 2016, Multisensory research.

[27]  A. Lindner,et al.  The valence of action outcomes modulates the perception of one’s actions , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[28]  Nina Schaffert,et al.  Effects of acoustic feedback training in elite-standard Para-Rowing , 2015, Journal of sports sciences.

[29]  Nina Schaffert,et al.  Interactive Sonification in Rowing: Acoustic Feedback for On-Water Training , 2015, IEEE MultiMedia.

[30]  Bruno L. Giordano,et al.  Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect , 2006, Cognition.

[31]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Do We Really Need Vision? How Blind People “See” the Actions of Others , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  A. King,et al.  The shape of ears to come: dynamic coding of auditory space , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  Karen B. Schloss,et al.  Music–color associations are mediated by emotion , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  Ophelia Deroy,et al.  Understanding the Correspondences: Introduction to the Special Issue on Crossmodal Correspondences. , 2016, Multisensory research.

[35]  James L. Alty,et al.  The rising pitch metaphor: an empirical study , 2005, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[36]  N. Bolognini,et al.  Visual search improvement in hemianopic patients after audio-visual stimulation. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  J. Wolfe,et al.  When the animal destroys the beach, the beach destroys the animal. Mutually assured destruction in gist processing , 2010 .

[38]  C. Spence,et al.  Crossmodal interactions in the perception of expressivity in musical performance , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[39]  Balaji Thoshkahna,et al.  Design and evaluation of the effectiveness of a sonification technique for real time heart-rate data , 2016, Journal on Multimodal User Interfaces.

[40]  G. Holstege,et al.  Emotions Studied by Imaging of the Human Brain: The Somatic and Emotional Motor Systems 67 , 2013 .

[41]  P. Niedenthal,et al.  Body and Mind: Zajonc’s (Re)introduction of the Motor System to Emotion and Cognition , 2010 .

[42]  C. Spence,et al.  Hedonic mediation of the crossmodal correspondence between taste and shape , 2015 .

[43]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Motor prediction , 2001, Current Biology.

[44]  D. Levine,et al.  Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[45]  Ana Tajadura-Jiménez,et al.  When room size matters: acoustic influences on emotional responses to sounds. , 2010, Emotion.

[46]  Marta Olivetti Belardinelli,et al.  How and when auditory action effects impair motor performance , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Anne Treisman,et al.  Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[48]  Nadia Bolognini,et al.  Visual search improvement in hemianopic patients after audio-visual stimulation , 2005 .

[49]  Julian F. Henriques Sonic Dominance and the Reggae Sound System Session , 2003 .

[50]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Changing pitch induced visual motion illusion , 2004, Current Biology.

[51]  W. Penfield The Cerebral Cortex of Man , 1950 .

[52]  D. R. Dechaine Affect and embodied understanding in musical experience , 2002 .

[53]  L. Trainor,et al.  Feeling the Beat: Movement Influences Infant Rhythm Perception , 2005, Science.

[54]  Cesare V Parise,et al.  Crossmodal Correspondences: Standing Issues and Experimental Guidelines. , 2016, Multisensory research.

[55]  F. Martin,et al.  Musical chords and emotion: Major and minor triads are processed for emotion , 2015, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[56]  D. Anton Occupational biomechanics , 1986 .

[57]  Thomas A. Farmer,et al.  Hand in Motion Reveals Mind in Motion , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[58]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense? , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[59]  R. Wilcox Introduction to Robust Estimation and Hypothesis Testing , 1997 .

[60]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affect and cognition: The hard interface. , 1985 .

[61]  H. Heffner,et al.  Visual factors in sound localization in mammals , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[62]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[63]  Marc O Ernst,et al.  Natural auditory scene statistics shapes human spatial hearing , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  Helder Bértolo,et al.  Visual imagery without visual perception: lessons from blind subjects , 2014, Other Conferences.

[65]  M. Sa,et al.  SPATIAL STEREOTYPES OF FOUR DIMENSIONS OF PURE TONE. , 1963 .

[66]  Cecilia Heyes,et al.  Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  R. Zatorre,et al.  When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[68]  J. Maga,et al.  Influence of Color on Taste Thresholds , 1974 .

[69]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.

[70]  Renee Timmers,et al.  Beethoven’s last piano sonata and those who follow crocodiles: Cross-domain mappings of auditory pitch in a musical context , 2010, Cognition.

[71]  Sean R. Mitchell,et al.  A novel, real-time biomechanical feedback system for use in rowing , 2014 .

[72]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Auditory rhythms are systemically associated with spatial-frequency and density information in visual scenes , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[73]  Ana Tajadura-Jiménez,et al.  I-Space: The Effects of Emotional Valence and Source of Music on Interpersonal Distance , 2011, PloS one.

[74]  J. van der Kamp,et al.  The Power of Auditory-Motor Synchronization in Sports: Enhancing Running Performance by Coupling Cadence with the Right Beats , 2013, PloS one.

[75]  S. Aglioti,et al.  Representing actions through their sound , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[77]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The Temporal Evolution of Feedback Gains Rapidly Update to Task Demands , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[78]  Barry R. Parker,et al.  Good Vibrations: The Physics of Music , 2009 .

[79]  C. Spence,et al.  Crossmodal Mental Imagery , 2013 .

[80]  Jonathan B Freeman,et al.  MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[81]  Kevin C. Dieter,et al.  Kinesthesis Can Make an Invisible Hand Visible , 2014, Psychological science.

[82]  C J Vierck,et al.  A critical band filter in touch , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[83]  Nina Schaffert,et al.  An investigation of online acoustic information for elite rowers in on-water training conditions , 2011 .

[84]  Nao Ninomiya,et al.  The 10th anniversary of journal of visualization , 2007, J. Vis..

[85]  V. Yohai HIGH BREAKDOWN-POINT AND HIGH EFFICIENCY ROBUST ESTIMATES FOR REGRESSION , 1987 .

[86]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Why the Sunny Side Is Up , 2004, Psychological science.

[87]  C. E. Osgood,et al.  Studies in Synesthetic Thinking: II. The Rôle Of Form In Visual Responses To Music , 1942 .

[88]  Giovanni Pezzulo,et al.  Tracking Second Thoughts: Continuous and Discrete Revision Processes during Visual Lexical Decision , 2015, PloS one.

[89]  S. Sterbing-D’Angelo Evolution of Sound Localization in Mammals , 2007 .

[90]  Christine H. Müller,et al.  High Breakdown Point and High Efficiency , 1997 .

[91]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Imagery and spatial processes in blindness and visual impairment , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.