Circadian Preference Modulates the Neural Substrate of Conflict Processing across the Day

Human morning and evening chronotypes differ in their preferred timing for sleep and wakefulness, as well as in optimal daytime periods to cope with cognitive challenges. Recent evidence suggests that these preferences are not a simple by-product of socio-professional timing constraints, but can be driven by inter-individual differences in the expression of circadian and homeostatic sleep-wake promoting signals. Chronotypes thus constitute a unique tool to access the interplay between those processes under normally entrained day-night conditions, and to investigate how they impinge onto higher cognitive control processes. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we assessed the influence of chronotype and time-of-day on conflict processing-related cerebral activity throughout a normal waking day. Sixteen morning and 15 evening types were recorded at two individually adapted time points (1.5 versus 10.5 hours spent awake) while performing the Stroop paradigm. Results show that interference-related hemodynamic responses are maintained or even increased in evening types from the subjective morning to the subjective evening in a set of brain areas playing a pivotal role in successful inhibitory functioning, whereas they decreased in morning types under the same conditions. Furthermore, during the evening hours, activity in a posterior hypothalamic region putatively involved in sleep-wake regulation correlated in a chronotype-specific manner with slow wave activity at the beginning of the night, an index of accumulated homeostatic sleep pressure. These results shed light into the cerebral mechanisms underlying inter-individual differences of higher-order cognitive state maintenance under normally entrained day-night conditions.

[1]  A. Adamantidis,et al.  Physiological arousal: a role for hypothalamic systems , 2008, Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences.

[2]  J. Wolfe,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article SEARCHING NIGHT AND DAY: A Dissociation of Effects of Circadian Phase and Time Awake on Visual Selective Attention and Vigilance , 2022 .

[3]  Yan Zhu,et al.  A neural circuit for circadian regulation of arousal , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  A. Wirz-Justice,et al.  Life between Clocks: Daily Temporal Patterns of Human Chronotypes , 2003, Journal of biological rhythms.

[5]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[6]  M. J. Meloy,et al.  The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. , 2005, Sleep.

[7]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations , 1985 .

[8]  G. Vandewalle,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging-Assessed Brain Responses during an Executive Task Depend on Interaction of Sleep Homeostasis, Circadian Phase, and PER3 Genotype , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[10]  I Feinberg,et al.  Systematic trends across the night in human sleep cycles. , 1979, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Jin Fan,et al.  Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.

[12]  G. Kerkhof Differences between morning-types and evening-types in the dynamics of EEG slow wave activity during night sleep. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[13]  Thomas E. Scammell,et al.  The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  R Turner,et al.  Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T , 2004, NeuroImage.

[15]  André Charles,et al.  Effects of sleep deprivation on Color-Word, Emotional, and Specific Stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[16]  Katherine L. Roberts,et al.  Examining a Supramodal Network for Conflict Processing: A Systematic Review and Novel Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data for Related Visual and Auditory Stroop Tasks , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  D. Dinges,et al.  Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. , 2004, Sleep.

[18]  A. N. van den Pol,et al.  Neurons Containing Hypocretin (Orexin) Project to Multiple Neuronal Systems , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  D. Dijk,et al.  Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. , 1999, Sleep research online : SRO.

[20]  V. Mongrain,et al.  Increased homeostatic response to behavioral sleep fragmentation in morning types compared to evening types. , 2007, Sleep.

[21]  K. J. Parker,et al.  Circadian and Homeostatic Regulation of Hypocretin in a Primate Model: Implications for the Consolidation of Wakefulness , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  E. Mignot,et al.  Lesions of the suprachiasmatic nucleus eliminate the daily rhythm of hypocretin-1 release. , 2004, Sleep.

[23]  Julie Carrier,et al.  Difference in sleep regulation between morning and evening circadian types as indexed by antero‐posterior analyses of the sleep EEG , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  M. Mühlethaler,et al.  Selective Action of Orexin (Hypocretin) on Nonspecific Thalamocortical Projection Neurons , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  J. Mattout,et al.  Automatic analysis of single-channel sleep EEG: validation in healthy individuals. , 2007, Sleep.

[26]  Lisa Y. M. Chuah,et al.  Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition , 2008, Current opinion in neurology.

[27]  D. Dijk,et al.  PER3 Polymorphism Predicts Sleep Structure and Waking Performance , 2007, Current Biology.

[28]  M. Hogge,et al.  Exploration of the neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging , 2006, Neuroscience.

[29]  I. Feinberg,et al.  Kinetics of non-rapid eye movement delta production across sleep and waking in young and elderly normal subjects: theoretical implications. , 2003, Sleep.

[30]  Christophe Phillips,et al.  fMRI Artefact Rejection and Sleep Scoring Toolbox , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[31]  Johanna H Meijer,et al.  Long term effects of sleep deprivation on the mammalian circadian pacemaker. , 2007, Sleep.

[32]  J. Horne,et al.  A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. , 1976, International journal of chronobiology.

[33]  Mariska J Vansteensel,et al.  Sleep states alter activity of suprachiasmatic nucleus neurons , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  E. Wolpert A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects. , 1969 .

[35]  Peter Achermann,et al.  Individual ‘Fingerprints’ in Human Sleep EEG Topography , 2001, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[36]  G. A. Kerkkof Differences between morning-types and evening-types in the dynamics of EEG slow wave activity during night sleep , 1991 .

[37]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Association of intrinsic circadian period with morningness-eveningness, usual wake time, and circadian phase. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[38]  E. Mignot,et al.  Fluctuation of extracellular hypocretin‐1 (orexin A) levels in the rat in relation to the light–dark cycle and sleep–wake activities , 2001, European Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[40]  M. Chee,et al.  The Neural Basis of Interindividual Variability in Inhibitory Efficiency after Sleep Deprivation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[42]  A. Wirz-Justice,et al.  A relationship between heat loss and sleepiness: effects of postural change and melatonin administration. , 1997, Journal of applied physiology.

[43]  P. Geisler,et al.  Hypothalamic gray matter changes in narcoleptic patients , 2002, Nature Medicine.

[44]  A. Rechtschaffen,et al.  A manual of standardized terminology, technique and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects , 1968 .

[45]  M. Mühlethaler,et al.  Exclusive Postsynaptic Action of Hypocretin-Orexin on Sublayer 6b Cortical Neurons , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Homeostatic Sleep Pressure and Responses to Sustained Attention in the Suprachiasmatic Area , 2009, Science.

[47]  R. Salomon,et al.  Diurnal variation of cerebrospinal fluid hypocretin-1 (Orexin-A) levels in control and depressed subjects , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.