Dissociating stimulus-set and response-set in the context of task-set switching.

The primary aim of the present research was to determine how stimulus-set and response-set components of task-set contribute to switch costs and conflict processing. Three experiments are described wherein participants completed an explicitly cued task-switching procedure. Experiment 1 established that task switches requiring a reconfiguration of both stimulus- and response-set incurred larger residual switch costs than task switches requiring the reconfiguration of stimulus-set alone. Between-task interference was also drastically reduced for response-set conflict compared with stimulus-set conflict. A second experiment replicated these findings and demonstrated that stimulus- and response-conflict have dissociable effects on the "decision time" and "motor time" components of total response time. Finally, a third experiment replicated Experiment 2 and demonstrated that the stimulus- and response- components of task switching and conflict processing elicit dissociable neural activity as evidence by event-related brain potentials.

[1]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  A C Roberts,et al.  Primate analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  A. Crider Perseveration in schizophrenia. , 1997, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[4]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  W. Perlstein,et al.  Increased Stroop facilitation effects in schizophrenia are not due to increased automatic spreading activation , 1999, Schizophrenia Research.

[6]  G. S. Klein,et al.  SEMANTIC POWER MEASURED THROUGH THE INTERFERENCE OF WORDS WITH COLOR-NAMING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[7]  T. Robbins,et al.  A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Conflict and Cognitive Control in the Brain , 2006 .

[9]  S. R. Jackson,et al.  Using advance information in dynamic cognitive control: An ERP study of task-switching , 2006, Brain Research.

[10]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: Effects of frequency, sequence, and conflict , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  G. Wylie,et al.  Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. , 1999 .

[12]  Eduardo Martínez-Montes,et al.  False discovery rate and permutation test: An evaluation in ERP data analysis , 2010, Statistics in medicine.

[13]  T. Robbins,et al.  Specific Neuropsychological Deficits in Schizophrenic Patients with Preserved Intellectual Function , 1998 .

[14]  Frini Karayanidis,et al.  ERPs dissociate the effects of switching task sets and task cues , 2006, Brain Research.

[15]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A functional MRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[16]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory and poststimulus components of task switching. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[17]  D. Besner,et al.  On a variant of Stroop’s paradigm: Which cognitions press your buttons? , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[18]  J. R. Simon,et al.  CHOICE REACTION TIME AS A FUNCTION OF ANGULAR STIMULUS-RESPONSE CORRESPONDENCE AND AGE , 1963 .

[19]  Moses M. Langley,et al.  An investigation of the neural correlates of attention and effector switching using ERPs , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[20]  Erik M. Altmann,et al.  Tasks of a feather flock together: Similarity effects in task switching , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[21]  Rachel Swainson,et al.  Fractionating the cognitive control required to bring about a change in task: A dense-sensor event-related potential study , 2008 .

[22]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[23]  D. A. Taylor,et al.  The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Disentangling Sequential Effects of Stimulus- and Response-related Conflict and Stimulus-Response Repetition using Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[26]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space , 1999 .

[27]  James J. Gibson,et al.  A critical review of the concept of set in contemporary experimental psychology. , 1941 .

[28]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Adaptive linear step-up procedures that control the false discovery rate , 2006 .

[30]  A. Jensen,et al.  The Stroop color-word test: a review. , 1966, Acta psychologica.

[31]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Sensitivity of electrophysiological activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and performance feedback. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Sharna Jamadar,et al.  Advance Preparation in Task-Switching: Converging Evidence from Behavioral, Brain Activation, and Model-Based Approaches , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[33]  G. Jackson,et al.  Behavioural and neurophysiological correlates of bivalent and univalent responses during task switching , 2007, Brain Research.

[34]  A C Nobre,et al.  Components of attentional set-switching. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[35]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[36]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The effects of recent practice on task switching. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  R. West,et al.  Sensitivity of medial frontal cortex to response and nonresponse conflict. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[38]  E. Neumann,et al.  Stroop theory, memory, and prefrontal cortical functioning: reply to Cohen et al. (1997) , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  I. Biederman,et al.  Mental set and mental shift revisited , 1976 .

[40]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[41]  Punit Shah Toward a Neurobiology of Unrealistic Optimism , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[42]  S. Kornblum,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  R. West,et al.  Neural correlates of cue retrieval, task set reconfiguration, and rule mapping in the explicit cue task switching paradigm. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[44]  Iring Koch,et al.  Sequential task predictability in task switching , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[45]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[46]  Paul D. Kieffaber,et al.  Event related brain potential evidence for preserved attentional set switching in schizophrenia , 2007, Schizophrenia Research.

[47]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[48]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[49]  Marcel Brass,et al.  When the same response has different meanings: recoding the response meaning in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[50]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Components of Switching Intentional Set , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  S Monsell,et al.  Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.

[52]  Shulan Hsieh,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of task conflicts in task-switching , 2008, Brain Research.

[53]  Antao Chen,et al.  Neural correlates of stimulus and response interference in a 2-1 mapping stroop task. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[54]  M. Walton,et al.  Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Paul D. Kieffaber,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of task switching and switch costs. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[56]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[57]  N. Cohen,et al.  The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[58]  Frank Rösler,et al.  Is task switching nothing but cue priming? Evidence from ERPs , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[59]  G. Ward,et al.  Task-Switching Costs, Stroop-Costs, and Executive Control: A Correlational Study , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[60]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Parametric Manipulation of the Conflict Signal and Control-state Adaptation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  Hilde Haider,et al.  Preparatory processes in the task-switching paradigm: evidence from the use of probability cues. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[62]  R Hübner,et al.  On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: evidence from two-component task shifts. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[63]  Two-Component Processes in Switching Attention: A Study of Event-Related Potentials , 2002, Perceptual and motor skills.

[64]  Deanna M Barch,et al.  Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: diagnostic specificity, 4-week course, and relationships to clinical symptoms. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[65]  Bruno Kopp,et al.  Fractionating the Neural Mechanisms of Cognitive Control , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  A. Jersild Mental set and shift , 2011 .

[67]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[68]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  On the role of stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect , 2003 .

[69]  A. Treisman,et al.  Attention, Space, and Action: Studies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .

[70]  N. Meiran,et al.  Task set switching in schizophrenia. , 2000, Neuropsychology.

[71]  Rachel Swainson,et al.  Fractionating the Cognitive Control Required to Bring About a Change in Task: A Dense-sensor Event-related Potential Study , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[72]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[73]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[74]  Paul D. Kieffaber,et al.  Switch and maintenance of task set in schizophrenia , 2006, Schizophrenia Research.

[75]  T. Goschke Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .

[76]  C. Carter,et al.  Attentional control and word inhibition in schizophrenia , 2002, Psychiatry Research.

[77]  Nicholas Wymbs,et al.  Neural correlates of conflict processing , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[78]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .