Self-assessment, self-direction, and the self-regulating professional.

One of the cornerstones of autonomy for any profession is the claim to self-regulation. To be effectively self-regulating, the profession generally depends on the individual practitioner to self-regulate his own maintenance of competence activities. This model of individual self-regulation, in turn, depends on the practitioner's ability to self-assess gaps in competence and willingness to seek out opportunities to redress these gaps when identified. The literature relevant to these processes, however, would suggest this model of individual self-regulation is overly optimistic. We review the literature and describe several difficulties associated with the traditionally held model of individual self-regulation. In particular, research demonstrates repeatedly that 1) self-assessment is not an effective mechanism to identify areas of personal weakness and that 2) even when areas of weakness are obvious to the adult learner, we often avoid engaging in learning in these areas because such learning often takes more energy and commitment than we are willing to expend. Implications of these difficulties for the current model of self-regulation are explored.

[1]  Glenn Regehr,et al.  Measuring Self-assessment: Current State of the Art , 2002, Advances in health sciences education : theory and practice.

[2]  M. Knowles The adult learner : a neglected species , 1979 .

[3]  Stephen H. Miller American board of medical specialties and repositioning for excellence in lifelong learning: Maintenance of certification , 2005, The Journal of continuing education in the health professions.

[4]  P. Benson,et al.  Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1993). Surpassing ourselves: An enquiry into the nature and implications of expertise. Chicago and La Salle, Illinois: Open Court Publications. , 2006 .

[5]  J. Suls,et al.  Flawed Self-Assessment , 2004, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[6]  K. Eva Stemming the tide: cognitive aging theories and their implications for continuing education in the health professions. , 2003, The Journal of continuing education in the health professions.

[7]  D. Boud,et al.  Student Self-Assessment in Higher Education: A Meta-Analysis , 1989 .

[8]  I C McManus,et al.  Linking assessment to learning: a new route to quality assurance in medical practice , 2002, Medical education.

[9]  J. Kruger,et al.  Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Robert J. Crutcher,et al.  The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. , 1993 .

[11]  Kevin W Eva,et al.  The Aging Physician: Changes in Cognitive Processing and Their Impact on Medical Practice , 2002, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[12]  M. J. Gordon A review of the validity and accuracy of self‐assessments in health professions training , 1991, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[13]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and related domains. , 2004, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[14]  M. Scardamalia,et al.  Surpassing Ourselves: An Inquiry into the Nature and Implications of Expertise , 1993 .

[15]  R. Cruess,et al.  "Profession": A Working Definition for Medical Educators , 2004, Teaching and learning in medicine.