Gestures accompanying speech in specifically language-impaired children and their timing with speech

The repertoire and timing of gestures accompanying speech were compared in children with specific language impairment (SLI), aged 5—10 years, in typically developing peers (CA), individually matched on age and nonverbal IQ, and in younger language-matched (LM) children. They were videotaped in two tasks, recounting a cartoon and describing their classroom. Three types of gestures were coded — iconics, deictics and beats — and the synchrony of these gestures with speech was examined in terms of number of words encompassed, grammatical speech category at gesture onset, and relationship of iconic gestures to the concept expressed in speech. All groups used more deictic gestures in the classroom description task. SLI children differed from the comparison children only in their use of iconic gestures. They produced somewhat more of these, used them more often to replace words, and began them more often on a noun phrase object. Otherwise, language proficiency, at least as measured by standardized tests, did not appear to impact the gestural system. The fact that, for all groups, most iconic and deictic gestures began on the noun phrase subject indicates a close synchrony between gesture and speech onset.

[1]  S. E. Weismer,et al.  The influence of prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment. , 1993, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[2]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The role of gesture in communication and thinking , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  George McCloskey,et al.  Leiter International Performance Scale , 1987 .

[4]  Maurizio Gentilucci,et al.  Speech and gesture share the same communication system , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  S Goldin-Meadow,et al.  What's communication got to do with it? Gesture in children blind from birth. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[6]  S. Nobe Language and Gesture: Where do most spontaneous representational gestures actually occur with respect to speech? , 2000 .

[7]  Spontaneous measures of morphosyntax in children with specific language impairment , 2004 .

[8]  Elena Nicoladis,et al.  A cross-cultural study on the use of gestures: Evidence for cross-linguistic transfer? , 2006 .

[9]  A. Kendon Some Relationships Between Body Motion and Speech , 1972 .

[10]  B. Everitt,et al.  Statistical methods for rates and proportions , 1973 .

[11]  M. Studdert-Kennedy Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. , 1994 .

[12]  M G Riseborough Meaning in movement: an investigation into the interrelationship of physiographic gestures and speech in seven-year-olds. , 1982, British journal of psychology.

[13]  D. McNeill,et al.  A straight path-to where? Reply to Butterworth and Hadar. , 1989, Psychological review.

[14]  Akiba A. Cohen,et al.  The Communicative Functions of Hand I1lustrators , 1977 .

[15]  J. Streeck Gesture as communication I: Its coordination with gaze and speech , 1993 .

[16]  Morton Wiener,et al.  Age-related changes in hand and arm movements as nonverbal communication: Some conceptualizations and an empirical exploration. , 1975 .

[17]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Why people gesture when they speak , 1998, Nature.

[18]  Laura A. Petitto,et al.  On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American sign language , 1987, Cognition.

[19]  E. Nicoladis Some Gestures Develop in Conjunction with Spoken Language Development and Others Don't: Evidence from Bilingual Preschoolers , 2002 .

[20]  A. Kendon Gesticulation and Speech: Two Aspects of the Process of Utterance , 1981 .

[21]  Adam Kendon,et al.  Language and Gesture: Language and gesture: unity or duality? , 2000 .

[22]  J. D. Ruiter The production of gesture and speech , 2000 .

[23]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Pointing Toward Two-Word Speech in Young Children , 2003 .

[24]  R. Krauss,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article GESTURE, SPEECH, AND LEXICAL ACCESS: The Role of Lexical Movements in Speech Production , 2022 .

[25]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The two faces of gesture: Language and thought , 2005 .

[26]  Sotaro Kita,et al.  On-line Integration of Semantic Information from Speech and Gesture: Insights from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Alibali,et al.  Gesture-Speech Mismatch and Mechanisms of Learning: What the Hands Reveal about a Child′s State of Mind , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  Sotaro Kita,et al.  What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking , 2003 .

[29]  R. Mayberry,et al.  Gesture and early bilingual development. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[30]  Susan Duncan,et al.  Growth points in thinking-for-speaking , 1998 .

[31]  S Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Silence is liberating: removing the handcuffs on grammatical expression in the manual modality. , 1996, Psychological review.

[32]  Kawai Chui,et al.  Temporal patterning of speech and iconic gestures in conversational discourse , 2005 .

[33]  S. Kita Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet , 2003 .

[34]  Helle Iben Bylander,et al.  Reynell Developmental Language Scales , 2009 .

[35]  D. McNeill,et al.  Speech-gesture mismatches: Evidence for one underlying representation of linguistic and nonlinguistic information , 1998 .

[36]  Ann-Christin Månsson,et al.  The use of gestures as a compensatory strategy in adults with acquired aphasia compared to children with specific language impairment (SLI) , 1998, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[37]  M. C. Caselli,et al.  Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down's syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. , 2003, International journal of language & communication disorders.