The role of sensory information in the production of periodic finger-tapping sequences

A subject lacking proprioceptive and tactile sensibility below the neck and a group of control subjects performed sequences of periodic finger taps involving a pattern of accentuation. The required intertap interval was 700 ms. In some situations, the taps were synchronized with the clicks of a metronome. Feedback conditions were manipulated by either allowing or not allowing the subjects to hear the taps and see their finger movements. We recorded the trajectory of the subjects' finger displacement in the vertical plane, and the force and moment of contact of the finger with the response key. The control subjects achieved precise timing of the finger taps by trading off downstroke onset for movement duration, e.g., they initiated shorter-duration tapping movements with a delay. This strategy did not vary depending on task demands (e.g., synchronization) or feedback conditions. The deafferented patient produced intertap intervals on average close to the required value. However, his tap timing was characterized by increased variability and severe distortion (lengthening) after the accentuated tap, regardless of feedback conditions. He did not manifest the compensatory strategy whereby, in control subjects, movement onset was adjusted to movement duration. Thus, such a strategy in controls seems to depend on intact proprioceptive and/or tactile information from the moving limb. Upon withdrawal of visual and acoustic feedback, the deafferented subject increased the force of the taps and the amplitude of tapping movements; his mean synchronization error with the metronome also increased. However, he did not lose correct phasing between the taps and the clicks of the metronome. These findings suggest that, under normal circumstances, sequential movements are timed by an internal timekeeper which paces sensory consequences relating to the occurrence of behaviorally important events (e.g., finger taps), and not the onset of the movements eliciting those events. In the synchronization task, the timekeeper may be phase locked to the periodic acoustic stimuli by direct entrainment. Feedback information may be needed, however, for keeping any synchronization error as small as possible.

[1]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Underlying Time Perception with Some Estimates of its Characteristic Frequency , 1990, Perception.

[2]  J C Rothwell,et al.  Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses , 1973 .

[4]  G. E. Stelmach,et al.  The Timing Effects of Accent Production in Periodic Finger-Tapping Sequences. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  J. Bossom Movement without proprioception. , 1974, Brain research.

[7]  Alan M. Wing,et al.  Modeling variability and dependence in timing , 1996 .

[8]  D. Hary,et al.  Synchronizing human movement with an external clock source , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[9]  F. W. Mott,et al.  Experiments on the Influence of Sensory Nerves upon Movement and Nutrition of the Limbs. , 1895 .

[10]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Timing and time perception. , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[11]  C Voillaume,et al.  Modèles pour l'étude de la régulation des mouvements cadencés , 1971 .

[12]  R M Church,et al.  Properties of the Internal Clock a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Paul Fraisse,et al.  Les repères du sujet dans la synchronisation et dans la pseudo-synchronisation , 1971 .

[14]  Jirí Mates,et al.  A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence , 1994, Biological Cybernetics.

[15]  D Hary,et al.  Temporal tracking and synchronization strategies. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[16]  A Faulkner,et al.  On the Relation between Time Perception and the Timing of Motor Action: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Controlling the Timing of Movement , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[17]  J. Cole,et al.  Evoked potentials in a man with a complete large myelinated fibre sensory neuropathy below the neck. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[18]  A. Wing 28 The Long and Short of Timing in Response Sequences , 1980 .

[19]  H.-H. Schulze,et al.  The Error Correction Model for the Tracking of a Random Metronome: Statistical Properties and an Empirical Test , 1992 .

[20]  Shaffer Lh Rhythm and timing in skill. , 1982 .

[21]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  TilDing by Skilled Musicians , 1982 .

[22]  Jirí Mates,et al.  A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  A M Wing,et al.  Perturbations of auditory feedback delay and the timing of movement. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  M. Billon,et al.  The timing effects of accent production in synchronization and continuation tasks performed by musicians and nonmusicians , 1995, Psychological research.

[25]  L. Shaffer Rhythm and timing in skill. , 1982, Psychological review.

[26]  E. Sedgwick,et al.  The perceptions of force and of movement in a man without large myelinated sensory afferents below the neck. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.