Early Impulse Control: Treatment of Potential Errors within Pre-Programming and Control

Abstract Early aiming adjustments following an online perturbation are made possible by impulse control. This process may unfold even earlier when perturbations impose a greater risk of a costly overshoot error. Participants executed upward and downward aims to mediate the cost of potential errors—downward overshoots require more energy to correct against gravity. On 33% of the trials, texture elements on the aiming surface were shifted following onset to appear congruent or incongruent with the aiming direction, and consequently generate a misperception of the limb moving slower or faster, respectively. Thus, the risk of potential errors could be influenced by the online perturbation (e.g., increased perceived likelihood of overshooting following the incongruent background). Findings indicated greater undershooting for down compared to up, which reflects the principle of movement optimisation. There was also more undershooting for an incongruent compared to congruent background, which is consistent with early online adjustments counter-acting the misperceived limb velocity. However, there were no interactions throughout the movement trajectory. We suggest that while the initial pre-programme considers the cost of potential errors (target direction), early impulse control fails to discriminate the likelihood of these errors occurring following an online perturbation (moving background).

[1]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[2]  Digby Elliott,et al.  The effects of response priming on the planning and execution of goal-directed movements in the presence of a distracting stimulus. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[3]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Origins of submovements during pointing movements. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[4]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[5]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  The Utility of Vision During Action: Multiple Visuomotor Processes? , 2013, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  Lawrence E M Grierson,et al.  Goal-directed aiming and the relative contribution of two online control processes. , 2009, The American journal of psychology.

[8]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Spatiotemporal Tuning of Rapid Interactions between Visual-Motion Analysis and Reaching Movement , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  James L. Lyons,et al.  The influence of target context and early and late vision on goal-directed reaching , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Lawrence E M Grierson,et al.  The Influence of Visual Feedback and Prior Knowledge About Feedback on Vertical Aiming Strategies , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[11]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of advance information about target location and visual feedback on movement planning and execution. , 2006, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[12]  J. Duysens,et al.  Postural responses to target jumps and background motion in a fast pointing task , 2018, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Steve Hansen,et al.  Optimizing rapid aiming behaviour: movement kinematics depend on the cost of corrective modifications , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  James L. Lyons,et al.  The multiple process model of goal-directed reaching revisited , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object , 2003, Nature.

[17]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  Movement strategies in vertical aiming of older adults , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[19]  James L. Lyons,et al.  The Impact of Strategic Trajectory Optimization on Illusory Target Biases During Goal-Directed Aiming , 2016, Journal of motor behavior.

[20]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[21]  Lawrence E. M. Grierson,et al.  Kinematic analysis of goal-directed aims made against early and late perturbations: an investigation of the relative influence of two online control processes. , 2008, Human movement science.

[22]  Frédéric Crevecoeur,et al.  A perspective on multisensory integration and rapid perturbation responses , 2015, Vision Research.

[23]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Evidence for Continuous Processing of Visual Information in a Manual Video-Aiming Task , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  M. Posner,et al.  Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  Real-time manipulation of visual displacement during manual aiming. , 2008, Human movement science.

[26]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Chapter 1 Visual Processing Time and the Control of Movement , 1992 .

[27]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Goal-directed aiming: two components but multiple processes. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .

[29]  Romeo Chua,et al.  No automatic pilot for visually guided aiming based on colour , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[31]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[33]  H. Gomi Implicit online corrections of reaching movements , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[34]  Lawrence E M Grierson,et al.  Kinematic analysis of early online control of goal-directed reaches: a novel movement perturbation study. , 2009, Motor control.

[35]  Digby Elliott,et al.  The impact of real and illusory target perturbations on manual aiming , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Luc Tremblay,et al.  Learning to Optimize Speed, Accuracy, and Energy Expenditure: A Framework for Understanding Speed-Accuracy Relations in Goal-Directed Aiming , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[38]  Lawrence E M Grierson,et al.  The Impact of Real and Illusory Perturbations on the Early Trajectory Adjustments of Goal-Directed Movements , 2011, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  W D Beggs,et al.  The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[40]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.