Two social brains: neural mechanisms of intersubjectivity

It is the aim of this article to present an empirically justified hypothesis about the functional roles of the two social neural systems, namely the so-called ‘mirror neuron system’ (MNS) and the ‘mentalizing system’ (MENT, also ‘theory of mind network’ or ‘social neural network’). Both systems are recruited during cognitive processes that are either related to interaction or communication with other conspecifics, thereby constituting intersubjectivity. The hypothesis is developed in the following steps: first, the fundamental distinction that we make between persons and things is introduced; second, communication is presented as the key process that allows us to interact with others; third, the capacity to ‘mentalize’ or to understand the inner experience of others is emphasized as the fundamental cognitive capacity required to establish successful communication. On this background, it is proposed that MNS serves comparably early stages of social information processing related to the ‘detection’ of spatial or bodily signals, whereas MENT is recruited during comparably late stages of social information processing related to the ‘evaluation’ of emotional and psychological states of others. This hypothesis of MNS as a social detection system and MENT as a social evaluation system is illustrated by findings in the field of psychopathology. Finally, new research questions that can be derived from this hypothesis are discussed. This article is part of the themed issue ‘Physiological determinants of social behaviour in animals’.

[1]  K. Vogeley Communication as Fundamental Paradigm for Psychopathology , 2018, The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition.

[2]  Colin Studholme,et al.  Detecting default mode networks in utero by integrated 4D fMRI reconstruction and analysis , 2016, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Antoine Coutrot,et al.  Pupil dilation as an index of preferred mutual gaze duration , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.

[5]  V. Manera,et al.  Interpersonal predictive coding, not action perception, is impaired in autism , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  Marc Erich Latoschik,et al.  Hybrid Avatar-Agent Technology – A Conceptual Step Towards Mediated “Social” Virtual Reality and its Respective Challenges , 2015, i-com.

[7]  Pamela Baess,et al.  An Internet-Based Real-Time Audiovisual Link for Dual MEG Recordings , 2015, PloS one.

[8]  Meghan L. Meyer,et al.  The Default Mode of Human Brain Function Primes the Intentional Stance , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Kai Vogeley,et al.  Why we interact: On the functional role of the striatum in the subjective experience of social interaction , 2014, NeuroImage.

[10]  K. Vogeley,et al.  The Use of Virtual Characters to Assess and Train Non-Verbal Communication in High-Functioning Autism , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[11]  Jennifer A. Walsh,et al.  Strategies for Perceiving Facial Expressions in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2014, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[12]  Natacha S. Santos,et al.  Perceiving nonverbal behavior: Neural correlates of processing movement fluency and contingency in dyadic interactions , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[13]  Natacha S. Santos,et al.  Dissociating animacy processing in high-functioning autism: Neural correlates of stimulus properties and subjective ratings , 2014, Social neuroscience.

[14]  K. Vogeley,et al.  From gaze cueing to dual eye-tracking: Novel approaches to investigate the neural correlates of gaze in social interaction , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  Essa Yacoub,et al.  The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: An overview , 2013, NeuroImage.

[16]  Kai Vogeley,et al.  Neural correlates of “social gaze” processing in high-functioning autism under systematic variation of gaze duration☆ , 2013, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[17]  Kai Vogeley,et al.  A second-person neuroscience in interaction 1 , 2013, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  K. Vogeley A Social Cognitive Perspective on ‘Understanding' and ‘Explaining' , 2013, Psychopathology.

[19]  Antonia Hamilton,et al.  Recognition of Emotions in Autism: A Formal Meta-Analysis , 2013, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[20]  C. Frith,et al.  Social cognition in the we-mode , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  K. Yun On the Same Wavelength: Face-to-Face Communication Increases Interpersonal Neural Synchronization , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  K. Vogeley,et al.  Getting a Grip on Social Gaze: Control over Others’ Gaze Helps Gaze Detection in High-Functioning Autism , 2013, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[23]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The Busy Social Brain , 2013, Psychological science.

[24]  R. Hari,et al.  Emotions promote social interaction by synchronizing brain activity across individuals , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  J. Perner,et al.  Implicit and explicit theory of mind: state of the art. , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[26]  P. Fox,et al.  Introspective Minds: Using ALE Meta-Analyses to Study Commonalities in the Neural Correlates of Emotional Processing, Social & Unconstrained Cognition , 2012, PloS one.

[27]  S. Garrod,et al.  Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social world , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Xu Cui,et al.  NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation , 2012, NeuroImage.

[29]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Precursors to Social and Communication Difficulties in Infants At-Risk for Autism: Gaze Following and Attentional Engagement , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[30]  U. Alon,et al.  The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  Distinct Neural Systems Involved in Agency and Animacy Detection , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  D. Lakens,et al.  If They Move in Sync, They Must Feel in Sync: Movement Synchrony Leads to Attributions of Rapport and Entitativity , 2011 .

[33]  K. Vogeley,et al.  A Matter of Words: Impact of Verbal and Nonverbal Information on Impression Formation in High-Functioning Autism. , 2011 .

[34]  G. Fink,et al.  Minds Made for Sharing: Initiating Joint Attention Recruits Reward-related Neurocircuitry , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Animated brain: A functional neuroimaging study on animacy experience , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  Kai Vogeley,et al.  "Artificial humans": Psychology and neuroscience perspectives on embodiment and nonverbal communication , 2010, Neural Networks.

[37]  Line Garnero,et al.  Inter-Brain Synchronization during Social Interaction , 2010, PloS one.

[38]  U. Castiello,et al.  Toward You , 2010 .

[39]  G. Fink,et al.  It's in your eyes--using gaze-contingent stimuli to create truly interactive paradigms for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. , 2010, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[40]  J. Searle Making the Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization , 2010 .

[41]  Kai Vogeley,et al.  Responses to Nonverbal Behaviour of Dynamic Virtual Characters in High-Functioning Autism , 2010, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[42]  Kai Vogeley,et al.  Contextualising culture and social cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  M. Tomasello Why We Cooperate , 2009 .

[44]  U. Frith,et al.  Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome , 2009, Science.

[45]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Duration matters: Dissociating neural correlates of detection and evaluation of social gaze , 2009, NeuroImage.

[46]  Jason P. Mitchell Social psychology as a natural kind , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  F. Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. , 2009 .

[48]  K. Vogeley,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Mental Illness: Consciousness of oneself and others in relation to mental disorders , 2009 .

[49]  R. Adolphs,et al.  The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[50]  Uta Frith,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Processes in Social Cognition , 2008, Neuron.

[51]  Nicole David,et al.  Parametric induction of animacy experience , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[52]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Oliver Fischer,et al.  Deciphering the secret code: A new methodology for the cross-cultural analysis of nonverbal behavior , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[55]  Tyler S. Lorig,et al.  Social Neuroscience: Bridging Social and Biological Systems , 2008 .

[56]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[57]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[58]  R. Blake,et al.  Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[59]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis , 2007, Science.

[60]  Shawn C. Milleville,et al.  Understanding Animate Agents , 2007, Psychological science.

[61]  S. Blakemore,et al.  The application of eye‐tracking technology in the study of autism , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[62]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain , 2007, Nature.

[63]  C. Keysers,et al.  Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[64]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[65]  Janine Willis,et al.  First Impressions , 2006, Psychological science.

[66]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[67]  R. Poldrack Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[68]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[71]  H. Asperger,et al.  Die „Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter , 1944, Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten.

[72]  Nalini Ambady,et al.  The Glimpsed World: Unintended Communication and Unintended Perception , 2005 .

[73]  S. Gallagher How the body shapes the mind , 2005 .

[74]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[75]  K. Zilles,et al.  Neural Correlates of First-Person Perspective as One Constituent of Human Self-Consciousness , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  C. Frith,et al.  Schizophrenia and theory of mind , 2004, Psychological Medicine.

[77]  Jean Decety,et al.  When the self represents the other: A new cognitive neuroscience view on psychological identification , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[78]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[79]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.

[80]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  It takes two to mimic: behavioral consequences of self-construals. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[81]  K. Shockley,et al.  Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[83]  F. Volkmar,et al.  The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[84]  J. Piven,et al.  Visual Scanning of Faces in Autism , 2002, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[85]  Mukesh Dhamala,et al.  Hyperscanning : Simultaneous fMRI during Linked Social Interactions , 2001 .

[86]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[87]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[88]  Hiromitsu Kobayashi,et al.  Unique morphology of the human eye and its adaptive meaning: comparative studies on external morphology of the primate eye. , 2001, Journal of human evolution.

[89]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[90]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[91]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[92]  N. Emery,et al.  The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[93]  Gary Bente,et al.  Sex Differences in Body Movement and Visual Attention: An Integrated Analysis of Movement and Gaze in Mixed-Sex Dyads , 1998 .

[94]  M. Argyle,et al.  Gaze and Mutual Gaze , 1994, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[95]  H. Markus,et al.  Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. , 1991 .

[96]  T. Nagel The view from nowhere , 1987 .

[97]  A. Leslie Spatiotemporal Continuity and the Perception of Causality in Infants , 1984, Perception.

[98]  Kenneth Mark Colby,et al.  Clinical artificial intelligence , 1981, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[99]  M. Haith,et al.  Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. , 1978, Science.

[100]  M. Argyle,et al.  The Different Functions of Gaze , 1973 .

[101]  M. Argyle,et al.  The Communication of Inferior and Superior Attitudes by Verbal and Non‐verbal Signals* , 1970 .

[102]  A. Mehrabian,et al.  Decoding of inconsistent communications. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[103]  H. Kelley Attribution theory in social psychology , 1967 .

[104]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[105]  D. G. Hays,et al.  Disturbed communication : the clinical assessment of normal and pathological communicative behavior , 1957 .

[106]  A. Michotte,et al.  La perception de la causalite , 1956 .

[107]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[108]  J. Mill,et al.  An examination of Sir William Hamilton's philosophy , 1979 .