Mindset changes lead to drastic impairments in rule finding

Rule finding is an important aspect of human reasoning and flexibility. Previous studies associated rule finding failure with past experience with the test stimuli and stable personality traits. We additionally show that rule finding performance is severely impaired by a mindset associated with applying an instructed rule. The mindset was established in Phase 1 (manipulation) of the experiment, before rule finding ability was assessed in Phase 2 (testing). The impairment in rule finding was observed even when Phase 1 involved executing a single trial (Experiment 2), and when entirely different stimuli and rules were used in the two phases of the experiment (Experiments 3-6). Experiments 4-6 show that applying an instructed rule in Phase 1 impaired subsequent (Phase 2) feedback evaluation, rule generation, and attention switching between rules, which are the three component processes involved in rule finding according to COVIS (Ashby, Alfonso-Reese, Turken, & Waldron, 1998).

[1]  P. Gollwitzer Action phases and mind-sets , 1990 .

[2]  Kentaro Fujita,et al.  Planning and the Implementation of Goals , 2004 .

[3]  E. Langer,et al.  The prevention of mindlessness. , 1987 .

[4]  Beryl Hesketh,et al.  Dilemmas in training for transfer and retention , 1997 .

[5]  William M. Smith,et al.  A Study of Thinking , 1956 .

[6]  S. Mednick,et al.  INCUBATION OF CREATIVE PERFORMANCE AND SPECIFIC ASSOCIATIVE PRIMING. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[7]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Ellen J. Langer,et al.  Mindful Learning , 2000, Deep Learning in a Disorienting World.

[9]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  From thinking about what might have been to sharing what we know: The effects of counterfactual mind-sets on information sharing in groups , 2004 .

[10]  F RESTLE,et al.  The selection of strategies in cue learning. , 1962, Psychological review.

[11]  Mark D. LeBlanc,et al.  Text Integration and Mathematical Connections: A Computer Model of Arithmetic Word Problem Solving , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  T. M. Amabile The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. , 1983 .

[13]  W. Wittmann,et al.  Investigating the paths between working memory, intelligence, knowledge, and complex problem solving performances via Brunswik-symmetry , 1999 .

[14]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  The debiasing effect of counterfactual mind-sets: Increasing the search for disconfirmatory information in group decisions , 2003 .

[15]  E. Higgins,et al.  Handbook of motivation and cognition : foundations of social behavior , 1991 .

[16]  Barry Schwartz,et al.  Reinforcement-Induced Behavioral Stereotypy: How Not To Teach People To Discover Rules , 1982 .

[17]  The nature of insight , 1996 .

[18]  R. Sternberg,et al.  The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. , 1998 .

[19]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  Wim Fias,et al.  Post-error slowing: An orienting account , 2009, Cognition.

[21]  R. Sternberg Styles of thinking. , 1996 .

[22]  B. Nijstad,et al.  Group creativity : An introduction , 2003 .

[23]  A. Luchins Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. , 1942 .

[24]  N. Chater,et al.  Similarity and rules: distinct? exhaustive? empirically distinguishable? , 1998, Cognition.

[25]  L. Mo,et al.  Schema induction in problem solving: a multidimensional analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  H. Swanson,et al.  A Subgroup Analysis of Working Memory in Children with Reading Disabilities , 2001, Journal of learning disabilities.

[27]  S. Los,et al.  On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .

[28]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  M. Levine A cognitive theory of learning: Research on hypothesis testing. , 1975 .

[30]  F. Ashby,et al.  The effects of concurrent task interference on category learning: Evidence for multiple category learning systems , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  Andrew W. Moore,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: A Survey , 1996, J. Artif. Intell. Res..

[32]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  The Prepared Mind , 2006, Psychological science.

[33]  S. Ohlsson,et al.  Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving , 1999 .

[34]  Robert J. Sternberg,et al.  On being an expert: a cost-benefit analysis , 1992 .

[35]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[36]  Richard D. Roberts,et al.  Learning and Individual Differences: Process, Trait and Content Determinants , 1999 .

[37]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Response selection and response execution in task switching: evidence from a go-signal paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[38]  N. Yeung,et al.  On the ERN and the significance of errors. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[39]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[40]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. , 1998, Psychological review.

[41]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[42]  Andrew G. Barto,et al.  Reinforcement learning , 1998 .

[43]  John R. Anderson,et al.  History of Success and Current Context in Problem Solving Combined Influences on Operator Selection , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  Paul B. Paulus,et al.  Group creativity : An introduction , 2003 .

[45]  K. Fujita,et al.  Mindsets and pre-conscious open-mindedness to incidental information , 2007 .

[46]  R. Heaton,et al.  Use of Neuropsychological tests to predict adult patients' everyday functioning. , 1981, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[47]  G. Bower,et al.  Attention in Learning: Theory and Research , 1968 .

[48]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[49]  E. A. Berg,et al.  A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. , 1948, The Journal of general psychology.

[50]  P. Boeck,et al.  Generation speed in Raven's progressive matrices test , 1999 .

[51]  W Todd Maddox,et al.  Disrupting feedback processing interferes with rule-based but not information-integration category learning , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[52]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .

[53]  Gary E. Raney,et al.  An eye movement study of insight problem solving , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[54]  Junichiro Yoshimoto,et al.  Control of exploitation-exploration meta-parameter in reinforcement learning , 2002, Neural Networks.

[55]  A. Galinsky,et al.  Thinking within the box: The relational processing style elicited by counterfactual mind-sets. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Task Switching: Mechanisms Underlying Rigid vs. Flexible Self-control , 2009 .

[57]  M. Runco Creativity: Theories and Themes: Research, Development, and Practice , 2006 .

[58]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  Counterfactuals as behavioral primes: Priming the simulation heuristic and consideration of alternatives. , 2000 .

[59]  J. Raven Advanced progressive matrices : sets I and II , 1962 .

[60]  Martin E. Knowles,et al.  Immediate and sustained effects of planning in a problem-solving task. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[61]  E. Langer,et al.  Premature cognitive commitment. , 1981, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[62]  L. Lippman A Short Report concerning the Einstellung Effect , 1996 .

[63]  Craig A. Kaplan,et al.  In search of insight , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[64]  J R Anderson,et al.  Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[65]  N. Meiran,et al.  On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[66]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[67]  David Klahr,et al.  Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[68]  K. Duncker,et al.  On problem-solving , 1945 .

[69]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[70]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[71]  R. Sternberg Advances in the psychology of human intelligence , 1982 .

[72]  Gwenn W. Gröndal,et al.  Meta-analytic procedures for social research , 1993 .

[73]  T. Ormerod,et al.  Information processing and insight: a process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems. , 2001 .

[74]  Colleen Seifert,et al.  Demystification of Cognitive Insight : Opportunistic Assimilation and the Prepared-Mind Perspective , 1995 .

[75]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[76]  M. Hewstone,et al.  Crossed Categorization and Intergroup Bias: The Moderating Roles of Intergroup and Affective Context , 2000 .

[77]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[78]  R. Sternberg,et al.  Intelligence, Information Processing and Analogical Reasoning : The Componential Analysis of Human Abilities , 1977 .

[79]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[80]  E. E. Jones,et al.  From Acts To Dispositions The Attribution Process In Person Perception1 , 1965 .

[81]  Stefanie Schuch,et al.  The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning , 1992, Handbook of Machine Learning.

[83]  R. Woodworth,et al.  PSYCHIATRY AND EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY , 1906 .

[84]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[85]  S. Monsell Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[86]  D. Willingham Cognition: The Thinking Animal , 2000 .

[87]  E. Torrance,et al.  The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking , 2012 .

[88]  T. Broadbent,et al.  Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge , 1972 .

[89]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[90]  J. Gustafsson Measuring and understanding G: Experimental and correlational approaches. , 1999 .

[91]  Jonathan Evans In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[92]  J. Lehto A Test for Children's Goal-Directed Behavior: A Pilot Study , 2004, Perceptual and motor skills.

[93]  K. Vohs,et al.  Handbook of self-regulation : research, theory, and applications , 2004 .

[94]  J. Guilford,et al.  The nature of human intelligence. , 1968 .

[95]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The effects of recent practice on task switching. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[96]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Dual-task interference in perceptual category learning , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[97]  D E Meyer,et al.  Activation and metacognition of inaccessible stored information: potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[98]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Book reviewIntelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: by Robert J. Sternberg. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1977. xi + 348 pp. $19.95 , 1977 .

[99]  Shigeru Sato,et al.  Neural activity in the human brain signals logical rule identification. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[100]  Michael E Atwood,et al.  A process model for water jug problems , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[101]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[102]  P. Costa,et al.  Declines in divergent thinking with age: cross-sectional, longitudinal, and cross-sequential analyses. , 1987, Psychology and aging.

[103]  Declines in divergent thinking with age: cross-sectional, longitudinal, and cross-sequential analyses. , 1987 .

[104]  C. Blair How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability. , 2006, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[105]  Günther Knoblich,et al.  Investigating the effect of mental set on insight problem solving. , 2008, Experimental psychology.

[106]  Robert J. Sternberg,et al.  Expertise and Intelligent Thinking: When is it Worse to Know Better , 1989 .

[107]  S. Bunge How we use rules to select actions: A review of evidence from cognitive neuroscience , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[108]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  From Thinking About What Might Have Been to Sharing What We Know: The Role of Counterfactual Mind-Sets in Information Sharing in Groups , 2002 .

[109]  Imre Lakatos,et al.  Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge , 1972 .

[110]  F. Heider,et al.  Principles of topological psychology , 1936 .

[111]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  Self control in society, mind, and brain , 2010 .

[112]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .