The role of emotion in moral socialization

will discuss the relationship of that research to some of the more enduring theoretical issues in emotion. Theories of emotion wax and wane across a more extended time frame than is typical for theoretical approaches in many other areas in psychology. Perhaps these extended cycles of emotion theory result from the relative difficulty in the recent past in doing definitive research within the area of human emotion. The field was thereby insulated from sudden shifts in focus precipitated by dramatic research relevant to basic theory. One of those areas of enduring concern to philosophers and psychologists interested in human emotion has been whether to conceive of emotion as dimensional versus discrete in structure. Those on the discrete side of this controversy generally trace their theoretical lineage to Charles Darwin, whereas dimensional theorists often credit William James. Discrete theorists hold that there are eight or nine basic emotions, which are inborn syndromes of feeling and behavior (e.g

[1]  D. Dutton,et al.  Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high anxiety. , 1974, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  C. Izard The face of emotion , 1971 .

[3]  J. M. Olson,et al.  An expectancy-attribution model of the effects of placebos. , 1981, Psychological review.

[4]  D. Bem Self-Perception Theory , 1972 .

[5]  C. W. Hughes Emotion: Theory, Research and Experience , 1982 .

[6]  P. Ellsworth,et al.  The role of facial response in the experience of emotion. , 1979, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  J. Laird,et al.  Individual differences in self-attribution of emotion. , 1974 .

[8]  Silvan S. Tomkins,et al.  AFFECT AND BEHAVIOR: ANXIETY AS A NEGATIVE AFFECT. , 1965 .

[9]  R. Dienstbier,et al.  Cheating as a function of the labeling of natural arousa. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  J. Singer,et al.  Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. , 1962, Psychological review.

[11]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Facial, autonomic, and subjective components of emotion: the facial feedback hypothesis versus externalizer-internalizer distinction. , 1981, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  J. V. von Wright,et al.  Relationships between sex-related psychological characteristics during adolescence and catecholamine excretion during achievement stress. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[13]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[14]  Retrospective misattribution of a conditioned emotional response. , 1974, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  J. P. Scott THE FUNCTION OF EMOTIONS IN BEHAVIORAL SYSTEMS: A SYSTEMS THEORY ANALYSIS , 1980 .

[16]  W. M. Waid Skin conductance response to both signaled and unsignaled noxious stimulation predicts level of socialization. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  L. Forsman Habitual catecholamine excretion and its relation to habitual distress , 1980, Biological Psychology.

[18]  M Frankenhaeuser,et al.  Psychoneuroendocrine approaches to the study of emotion as related to stress and coping. , 1978, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[19]  M. Hoffman Moral Internalization: Current Theory And Research , 1977 .

[20]  D. Lindsley The role of nonspecific reticulo-thalamo- cortical systems in emotion , 1970 .

[21]  Philip G. Zimbardo,et al.  Affective consequences of inadequately explained physiological arousal. , 1979 .

[22]  M Frankenhaeuser,et al.  Temporal factors in sympatho-adrenomedullary activity following acute behavioral activation. , 1973, Biological psychology.

[23]  D. G. Perry,et al.  Social Learning, Causal Attribution, and Moral Internalization , 1983 .

[24]  G. Bower Mood and memory. , 1981, The American psychologist.

[25]  Christina Maslach,et al.  Negative emotional biasing of unexplained arousal. , 1979 .

[26]  Silvan S. Tomkins,et al.  The negative affects , 1963 .

[27]  Dolf Zillmann,et al.  Attribution of apparent arousal and proficiency of recovery from sympathetic activation affecting excitation transfer to aggressive behavior , 1974 .

[28]  G. Mandler Mind and Emotion , 1975 .

[29]  R. Plutchik The emotions: Facts, theories and a new model. , 1964 .

[30]  Menstruation, reattribution, and competence. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  A. Doob,et al.  Attribution and aggression: The misattribution of anger ☆ , 1978 .

[32]  J Cooper,et al.  Arousal as a necessary condition for attitude change following induced compliance. , 1978, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  S. Schachter,et al.  Epinephrine, chlorpromazine, and amusement. , 1962, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[34]  J. Hillman,et al.  An emotion-attribution approach to moral behavior: interfacing cognitive and avoidance theories of moral development. , 1975, Psychological review.

[35]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the "overjustification" hypothesis. , 1973 .

[36]  Stanley Schachter,et al.  Cognitive manipulation of pain , 1966 .

[37]  R. Dienstbier Emotion-attribution theory: establishing roots and exploring future perspectives. , 1978, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[38]  S. Mednick,et al.  Biosocial bases of criminal behavior , 1977 .

[39]  Samuel R. Friedman,et al.  Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects , 1968 .

[40]  D. Zillmann,et al.  Excitation transfer from physical exercise to subsequent aggressive behavior. , 1972 .

[41]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[42]  R. Dienstbier Attraction increases and decreases as a function of emotion-attribution and appropriate social cues , 1979 .

[43]  J. Bryant,et al.  Enhancement of experienced sexual arousal in response to erotic stimuli through misattribution of unrelated residual excitation. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  V. Konečni,et al.  The mediation of aggressive behavior: arousal level versus anger and cognitive labeling. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  Effect of the severity of threat on the devaluation of forbidden behavior. , 1963 .

[46]  F. Wertham The Criminal Personality. Vol. 1, A Profile for Change , 1977 .

[47]  N. Burch,et al.  Personality correlates of catecholamine excretion under stress. , 1967, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[48]  J. Laird,et al.  Remembering What You Feel: Effects of Emotion on Memory , 1982 .

[49]  L. Kahle,et al.  The impact of moral theories on cheating , 1980 .

[50]  B. Latané,et al.  Crime, cognition, and the autonomic nervous system. , 1964 .

[51]  D. Magnusson,et al.  Catecholamine output in school children as related to performance and adjustment. , 1973, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[52]  R. Dienstbier The role of anxiety and arousal attribution in cheating , 1972 .

[53]  J Rodin,et al.  Toward an attribution therapy: the reduction of fear through induced cognitive-emotional misattribution. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[54]  Charles D. Spielberger,et al.  Anxiety and behavior , 1966 .

[55]  L. Robins Biosocial Bases of Criminal Behavior , 1978 .

[56]  S. E. Samenow,et al.  The criminal personality , 1976 .

[57]  Gary Alan Fine,et al.  Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 17. , 1976 .

[58]  J Cooper,et al.  Dissonance and the pill: an attribution approach to studying the arousal properties of dissonance. , 1974, Journal of personality and social psychology.