Children's perception of gap affordances: bicycling across traffic-filled intersections in an immersive virtual environment.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] P. B. Pufall,et al. Perceiving Whether or Not the World Affords Stepping Onto and Over: A Developmental Study , 1992 .
[2] Marion A. Eppler,et al. Crawling versus walking infants' perception of affordances for locomotion over sloping surfaces. , 1993, Child development.
[3] David N. Lee,et al. A roadside simulation of road crossing for children , 1984 .
[4] James E. Cutting,et al. How we avoid collisions with stationary and moving objects. , 1995 .
[5] Peter Willemsen,et al. Ribbon networks for modeling navigable paths of autonomous agents in virtual urban environments , 2003, IEEE Virtual Reality, 2003. Proceedings..
[6] V Cavallo,et al. Visual Information and Skill Level in Time-To-Collision Estimation , 1988, Perception.
[7] Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al. Multiple information sources in interceptive timing , 1997 .
[8] David C Schwebel,et al. The role of parents and temperament on children's estimation of physical ability: links to unintentional injury prevention. , 2003, Journal of pediatric psychology.
[9] D. Thompson,et al. Epidemiology of bicycle injuries and risk factors for serious injury. , 1997, Injury prevention : journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention.
[10] E R Hoffmann,et al. Children's Estimates of Vehicle Approach Times , 1980, Human factors.
[11] F P Rivara,et al. Prevention of injuries to children and adolescents , 2002, Injury prevention : journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention.
[12] K. Adolph. Specificity of Learning: Why Infants Fall Over a Veritable Cliff , 2000, Psychological science.
[13] F. C. Bakker,et al. The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: perception of catchableness of fly balls. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] D. Schwebel,et al. Social and temperamental influences on children's overestimation of their physical abilities: links to accidental injuries. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[15] R. Siegler,et al. Development of Time, Speed, and Distance Concepts , 1979 .
[16] T. Pitcairn,et al. Individual differences in road crossing ability in young children and adults. , 2000, British journal of psychology.
[17] W Schiff,et al. Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.
[18] T. Pitcairn,et al. Errors in young children's decisions about traffic gaps: experiments with roadside simulations. , 1992, British journal of psychology.
[19] R. Baillargeon,et al. Task constraints and infant grip configurations. , 1989, Developmental psychobiology.
[20] Jodie M. Plumert,et al. Relations Between Children ' s Overestimation of Their Physical Abilities and Accident Proneness , 2004 .
[21] E J Gibson,et al. Detection of the traversability of surfaces by crawling and walking infants. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] A. Yonas,et al. Effective action by infants to contact objects by reaching and leaning. , 1993, Child development.
[23] C. Michaels,et al. To Cross or Not to Cross: The Effect of Locomotion on Street-Crossing Behavior , 1996 .
[24] D. Thompson,et al. Incidence of bicycle-related injuries in a defined population. , 1990, American journal of public health.
[25] A. Dannenberg,et al. INJURIES TO BICYCLISTS: A NATIONAL PERSPECTIVE , 1993 .
[26] K. Adolph,et al. Psychophysical assessment of toddlers' ability to cope with slopes. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[28] J. Loomis,et al. Immersive virtual environment technology as a basic research tool in psychology , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[29] F. C. Bakker,et al. Catching balls: how to get the hand to the right place at the right time. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] R B Isler,et al. Child pedestrians' crossing gap thresholds. , 1998, Accident; analysis and prevention.
[31] Does vision guide stair climbing? a developmental study , 1992 .
[32] J. Piaget. Child's Conception of Movement and Speed , 1970 .
[33] Paul A. Braren,et al. How We Avoid Collisions With Stationary and Moving Obstacles , 2004 .
[34] J W Nixon,et al. Children and bicycles: what is really happening? Studies of fatal and non-fatal bicycle injury. , 1995, Injury prevention : journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention.
[35] D. Schwebel,et al. Longitudinal and concurrent relations among temperament, ability estimation, and injury proneness. , 1999, Child development.
[36] F. Wilkening. Integrating velocity, time, and distance information: A developmental study , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.
[37] H. Pick,et al. Integrating basic and applied developmental research: a new model for the twenty-first century. , 2000, Child development.
[38] H. Ross,et al. Optic-Flow and Cognitive Factors in Time-to-Collision Estimates , 1983, Perception.
[39] P. Hancock,et al. The Perception of Arrival Time for Different Oncoming Vehicles at an Intersection , 1994 .
[40] C. Hofsten. Catching Skills in Infancy , 1983 .
[41] John van der Kamp,et al. Information in learning to co-ordinate and control movements: Is there a need for specificity of practice? , 2000 .
[42] P. Hancock,et al. Influence of Approach Angle on Estimates of Time-To-Contact , 1996 .
[43] D S Young,et al. Training children in road crossing skills using a roadside simulation. , 1987, Accident; analysis and prevention.
[44] F P Rivara,et al. Traumatic deaths of children in the United States: currently available prevention strategies. , 1985, Pediatrics.
[45] Peter Willemsen,et al. Directable behavior models for virtual driving scenarios , 1997 .