Hippocampus, theta, and spatial memory

The past 18 months have witnessed interesting developments in several areas of hippocampal research. First, the mechanisms of hippocampal theta are becoming clear, as is its role in spatial coding; each theta cycle appears to act as a clock mechanism against which the firing of the place cells can be timed. Second, there has been a continued strengthening in the support for the spatial theory of hippocampal function from single unit and lesion experiments; particularly important is the finding that the deficit in (non-spatial) delayed non-match to sample memory experiments in the monkey following medial temporal lobe damage stems from the part of the cortex which surrounds the hippocampus, and not from the hippocampus itself. Third, in contrast, it is proving more difficult than originally thought to show a causal relationship between long-term potentiation at the synaptic level and place learning-induced changes in hippocampal synapses.

[1]  C. Y. Yim,et al.  Intrinsic membrane potential oscillations in hippocampal neurons in vitro , 1991, Brain Research.

[2]  J. Konopacki,et al.  Cholinergically-induced theta-like oscillations in the hippocampal formation of freely moving cats. , 1992, Neuroreport.

[3]  E. Brazhnik,et al.  Acetylcholine, theta-rhythm and activity of hippocampal neurons in the rabbit—III. Cortical input , 1993, Neuroscience.

[4]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Computer simulation of carbachol‐driven rhythmic population oscillations in the CA3 region of the in vitro rat hippocampus. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  R. Muller,et al.  The positional firing properties of medial entorhinal neurons: description and comparison with hippocampal place cells , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  I Q Whishaw,et al.  Acquisition and retention by hippocampal rats of simple, conditional, and configural tasks using tactile and olfactory cues: implications for hippocampal function. , 1991, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  Mark Stewart,et al.  Effects of atropine on hippocampal theta cells and complex-spike cells , 1992, Brain Research.

[8]  B. H. Bland,et al.  The Role of the Septohippocampal Pathway in the Regulation of Hippocampal Field Activity and Behavior: Analysis by the Intraseptal Microinfusion of Carbachol, Atropine, and Procaine , 1993, Experimental Neurology.

[9]  R. Sainsbury,et al.  Alpha2 modulation of type 1 and type 2 hippocampal theta in the freely moving rat , 1993, Brain Research Bulletin.

[10]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Neuronal activity in the hippocampus during delayed non‐match to sample performance in rats: Evidence for hippocampal processing in recognition memory , 1992, Hippocampus.

[11]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and spatial learning in the morris swim task , 1993, Hippocampus.

[12]  L. Jarrard,et al.  A role for hippocampus in the utilization of hunger signals. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[13]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Hippocampectomized monkeys can remember one place but not two , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Brenda Milner,et al.  Memory for different aspects of complex visual scenes after unilateral temporal- or frontal-lobe resection , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  G. Lynch,et al.  Patterned stimulation at the theta frequency is optimal for the induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation , 1986, Brain Research.

[16]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Induction of long‐term potentiation at perforant path dentate synapses does not affect place learning or memory , 1993, Hippocampus.

[17]  R. Vertes,et al.  Dorsal raphe neurons: synchronous discharge with the theta rhythm of the hippocampus in the freely behaving rat. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  N. McNaughton,et al.  Supramammillary cell firing and hippocampal rhythmical slow activity. , 1991, Neuroreport.

[19]  R. Vertes PHA‐L analysis of projections from the supramammillary nucleus in the rat , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[20]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granular layer , 1993, Hippocampus.

[21]  P. Holland,et al.  Preserved configural learning and spatial learning impairment in rats with hippocampal damage , 1992, Hippocampus.

[22]  R. Morris,et al.  Cumulative long‐term potentiation in the rat dentate gyrus correlates with, but does not modify, performance in the water maze , 1993, Hippocampus.

[23]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm , 1993, Hippocampus.

[24]  O. Vinogradova,et al.  Acetylcholine, theta-rhythm and activity of hippocampal neurons in the rabbit—I. Spontaneous activity , 1993, Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Deacon,et al.  Critical determinants of nonspatial working memory deficits in rats with conventional lesions of the hippocampus or fornix. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[26]  J. Konopacki,et al.  Carbachol-induced rhythmic slow activity (theta) in cat hippocampal formation slices , 1992, Brain Research.

[27]  T. Ono,et al.  Spatial responsiveness of monkey hippocampal neurons to various visual and auditory stimuli , 1992, Hippocampus.

[28]  O. Vinogradova,et al.  Acetylcholine, theta-rhythm and activity of hippocampal neurons in the rabbit—IV. Sensory stimulation , 1993, Neuroscience.

[29]  M. McKERNAN,et al.  Hippocampal lesions do not impair negative patterning: a challenge to configural association theory. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  S. Oddie,et al.  In vivo intracellular correlates of hippocampal formation theta-on and theta-off cells , 1992, Brain Research.

[31]  B L McNaughton,et al.  Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. , 1993, Science.

[32]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Selective hippocampal lesions abolish the contextual specificity of latent inhibition and conditioning. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[33]  G. Lynch,et al.  Factors regulating the magnitude of long-term potentiation induced by theta pattern stimulation , 1992, Brain Research.

[34]  E. Murray,et al.  Monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) with rhinal cortex ablations succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hr intertrial intervals and fail at matching to sample despite double sample presentations. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Differential effects of age on subpopulations of hippocampal theta cells , 1992, Neurobiology of Aging.

[36]  D. Amaral,et al.  Lesions of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in the monkey produce long-lasting memory impairment in the visual and tactual modalities , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  T. Ono,et al.  Monkey hippocampal neuron responses to complex sensory stimulation during object discrimination , 1992, Hippocampus.

[38]  E. Hargreaves,et al.  An examination of the relations between hippocampal long‐term potentiation, kindling, afterdischarge, and place learning in the water maze , 1993, Hippocampus.

[39]  B. H. Bland,et al.  The extrinsic modulation of hippocampal theta depends on the coactivation of cholinergic and GABA-ergic medial septal inputs , 1992, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[40]  P. Andersen,et al.  Association between brain temperature and dentate field potentials in exploring and swimming rats. , 1993, Science.

[41]  A. Chiba,et al.  Continuous recognition of spatial and nonspatial stimuli in hippocampal-lesioned rats. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[42]  L. Jarrard On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.