Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight

Bad presentation of medical statistics such as the risks associated with a particular intervention can lead to patients making poor decisions on treatment. Particularly confusing are single event probabilities, conditional probabilities (such as sensitivity and specificity), and relative risks. How can doctors improve the presentation of statistical information so that patients can make well informed decisions? The science fiction writer H G Wells predicted that in modern technological societies statistical thinking will one day be as necessary for efficient citizenship as the ability to read and write. How far have we got, a hundred or so years later? A glance at the literature shows a shocking lack of statistical understanding of the outcomes of modern technologies, from standard screening tests for HIV infection to DNA evidence. For instance, doctors with an average of 14 years of professional experience were asked to imagine using the Haemoccult test to screen for colorectal cancer.1 2 The prevalence of cancer was 0.3%, the sensitivity of the test was 50%, and the false positive rate was 3%. The doctors were asked: what is the probability that someone who tests positive actually has colorectal cancer? The correct answer is about 5%. However, the doctors' answers ranged from 1% to 99%, with about half of them estimating the probability as 50% (the sensitivity) or 47% (sensitivity minus false positive rate). If patients knew about this degree of variability and statistical innumeracy they would be justly alarmed. Statistical innumeracy is often attributed to problems inside our minds. We disagree: the problem is not simply internal but lies in the external representation of information, and hence a solution exists. Every piece of statistical information needs a representation–that is, a form. Some forms tend to cloud minds, while others foster insight. We know of no medical institution that teaches …

[1]  W. Casscells,et al.  Interpretation by physicians of clinical laboratory results. , 1978, The New England journal of medicine.

[2]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[3]  K. Wallston,et al.  Compliance to health recommendations: a theoretical overview of message framing , 1988 .

[4]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  How to Improve Bayesian Reasoning Without Instruction: Frequency Formats , 1995 .

[5]  T. Fahey,et al.  Evidence based purchasing: understanding results of clinical trials and systematic reviews , 1995, BMJ.

[6]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty , 1996, Cognition.

[7]  Ingvar Andersson,et al.  An Overview of the Swedish Randomised Mammography Trials: Total Mortality Pattern and the Representivity of the Study Cohorts , 1996, Journal of medical screening.

[8]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences , 1998, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[9]  P Howden-Chapman,et al.  Does the frame affect the picture? A study into how attitudes to screening for cancer are affected by the way benefits are expressed , 1998, Journal of medical screening.

[10]  J. Ward,et al.  How risks of breast cancer and benefits of screening are communicated to women: analysis of 58 pamphlets , 1998, BMJ.

[11]  A. Kühberger,et al.  The Influence of Framing on Risky Decisions: A Meta-analysis. , 1998, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[12]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  The visual communication of risk. , 1999, Journal of the National Cancer Institute. Monographs.

[13]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  How can we help people make sense of medical data? , 1999, Effective clinical practice : ECP.

[14]  P. Todd,et al.  Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .

[15]  Shannon K. McDonnell,et al.  EFFICACY OF BILATERAL PROPHYLACTIC MASTECTOMY IN WOMEN WITH A FAMILY HISTORY OF BREAST CANCER , 1999 .

[16]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Communicating Statistical Information , 2000, Science.

[17]  A. Dobson,et al.  Disease impact number and population impact number: population perspectives to measures of risk and benefit. , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[18]  C. McHorney,et al.  Frequency or Probability? A Qualitative Study of Risk Communication Formats Used in Health Care , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[19]  Jean-François Rouet,et al.  Multimedia learning : cognitive and instructional issues , 2001 .

[20]  G. Elwyn,et al.  Presenting risk information--a review of the effects of "framing" and other manipulations on patient outcomes. , 2001, Journal of health communication.

[21]  G. Gigerenzer Reckoning with Risk : Learning to Live with Uncertainty , 2002 .

[22]  G. Elwyn,et al.  One hundred years ago: Should milk be boiled? , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[23]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Calculated Risks: How to Know When Numbers Deceive You , 2002 .

[24]  J. Paling Strategies to help patients understand risks , 2003, BMJ : British Medical Journal.