Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evident from a variable spatial mapping paradigm.

We investigated whether a representation of a visual target can be stored in memory and used to support the online control of reaching movements. To distinguish between the use of a stored target representation for movement planning versus online control, we employed a novel movement environment in which participants could not fully plan their action in advance of movement initiation; that is, the spatial mapping between the movement of a computer mouse and the on-screen movement of a cursor was randomly varied from trial to trial. As such, participants were required to use online control to reach the target position. Reaches were examined in full-vision and three memory-dependent conditions (0, 2, and 5 s of delay). Absolute constant error did not accumulate between full-vision and brief delay trials (i.e., the 0-s delay), suggesting a stored representation of the visual target can be used for online control of reaching given a sufficiently brief delay interval. Longer delay trials (2 and 5 s) were less accurate and more variable than brief delay trials; however, the residual accuracy of these memory-dependent actions suggests that the motor system may have access to a stored representation of the visual target for online control processes for upwards of 5 s following target occlusion.

[1]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  Online visual control of the arm. , 1998, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[2]  S. Chieffi,et al.  Visual illusion and action , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.

[4]  D. Elliott,et al.  A visual representation and the control of manual aiming movements. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[7]  R A Abrams,et al.  Rapid aimed limb movements: differential effects of practice on component submovements. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  J. Brebner Continuing and reversing the direction of responding movements: some exceptions to the so-called "psychological refractory period". , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Gaze anchoring to a pointing target is present during the entire pointing movement and is driven by a non-visual signal. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[11]  W D Beggs,et al.  The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[12]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[14]  M F Bradshaw,et al.  The Effect of Pre-Movement Delays on Pointing Accuracy in Middle Childhood , 1998, Perception.

[15]  Romeo Chua,et al.  On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics. , 1998 .

[16]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Grasping after a Delay Shifts Size-Scaling from Absolute to Relative Metrics , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  M. Lemay,et al.  A distance effect in a manual aiming task to remembered targets: a test of three hypotheses , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  A Baddeley,et al.  The fractionation of working memory. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  Julie Messier,et al.  Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[24]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors , 1994, Vision Research.

[25]  Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al.  Planning Reaches by Evaluating Stored Postures. , 1995 .

[26]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Paolo Viviani,et al.  Altering the visuomotor gain , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aiming. , 1988, Canadian journal of psychology.

[29]  L. Proteau,et al.  The role of peripheral and central visual information for the directional control of manual aiming movements. , 1999, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[30]  Réjean Plamondon,et al.  A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  E A Roy,et al.  The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. , 2001, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[32]  D Elliott,et al.  Influence of Spatial Mapping on Manual Aiming Asymmetries , 1998, Perceptual and motor skills.

[33]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[34]  D. Elliott,et al.  The role of target information on manual-aiming bias , 1995, Psychological Research.

[35]  Graham K. Kerr,et al.  Visuo-motor adaptation during inactivation of the cerebellar nuclei: A preliminary report , 1993 .

[36]  R Plamondon A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[37]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Visual Information: The Control of Aiming Movements , 1981 .

[38]  M. Goodale,et al.  The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  P Conti,et al.  Role of Structured Visual Field and Visual Reafference in Accuracy of Pointing Movements , 1980, Perceptual and motor skills.

[40]  Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al.  Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Rescaling an acquired discrete aiming movement: Specific or general motor learning? , 1997 .