Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evident from a variable spatial mapping paradigm.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A P Georgopoulos,et al. Online visual control of the arm. , 1998, Novartis Foundation symposium.
[2] S. Chieffi,et al. Visual illusion and action , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[3] M. Jeannerod,et al. Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.
[4] D. Elliott,et al. A visual representation and the control of manual aiming movements. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[5] J. F. Soechting,et al. Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[6] R A Abrams,et al. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.
[7] R A Abrams,et al. Rapid aimed limb movements: differential effects of practice on component submovements. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[8] J. Brebner. Continuing and reversing the direction of responding movements: some exceptions to the so-called "psychological refractory period". , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[9] H. Bekkering,et al. Gaze anchoring to a pointing target is present during the entire pointing movement and is driven by a non-visual signal. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[10] D. Elliott,et al. The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[11] W D Beggs,et al. The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. , 1972, Acta psychologica.
[12] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[13] C. Prablanc,et al. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.
[14] M F Bradshaw,et al. The Effect of Pre-Movement Delays on Pointing Accuracy in Middle Childhood , 1998, Perception.
[15] Romeo Chua,et al. On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics. , 1998 .
[16] M. A. Goodale,et al. Grasping after a Delay Shifts Size-Scaling from Absolute to Relative Metrics , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[17] Romeo Chua,et al. Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.
[18] Matthew Heath,et al. The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] M. Lemay,et al. A distance effect in a manual aiming task to remembered targets: a test of three hypotheses , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[21] A Baddeley,et al. The fractionation of working memory. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] Julie Messier,et al. Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[23] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[24] David L. Sparks,et al. Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors , 1994, Vision Research.
[25] Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al. Planning Reaches by Evaluating Stored Postures. , 1995 .
[26] H. Bekkering,et al. Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[27] Paolo Viviani,et al. Altering the visuomotor gain , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] D. Elliott,et al. The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aiming. , 1988, Canadian journal of psychology.
[29] L. Proteau,et al. The role of peripheral and central visual information for the directional control of manual aiming movements. , 1999, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[30] Réjean Plamondon,et al. A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.
[31] E A Roy,et al. The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. , 2001, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[32] D Elliott,et al. Influence of Spatial Mapping on Manual Aiming Asymmetries , 1998, Perceptual and motor skills.
[33] A. G. Feldman. Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .
[34] D. Elliott,et al. The role of target information on manual-aiming bias , 1995, Psychological Research.
[35] Graham K. Kerr,et al. Visuo-motor adaptation during inactivation of the cerebellar nuclei: A preliminary report , 1993 .
[36] R Plamondon. A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[37] Les G. Carlton,et al. Visual Information: The Control of Aiming Movements , 1981 .
[38] M. Goodale,et al. The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[39] P Conti,et al. Role of Structured Visual Field and Visual Reafference in Accuracy of Pointing Movements , 1980, Perceptual and motor skills.
[40] Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al. Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] Digby Elliott,et al. Rescaling an acquired discrete aiming movement: Specific or general motor learning? , 1997 .