Development of Quantitative Thinking Across Correlated Dimensions
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Kahneman,et al. The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.
[2] R. Adams,et al. Monocular Contrast Sensitivity in 3- to 36-Month-Old Human Infants , 1996, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.
[3] Geetha B. Ramani,et al. Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children's numerical knowledge through playing number board games. , 2008, Child development.
[4] Ty W. Boyer,et al. Development of proportional reasoning: where young children go wrong. , 2008, Developmental psychology.
[5] Daniel C. Hyde,et al. All Numbers Are Not Equal: An Electrophysiological Investigation of Small and Large Number Representations , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] Fei Xu,et al. Number sense in human infants. , 2005, Developmental science.
[7] H. McGurk. The role of object orientation in infant perception. , 1970, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.
[8] U. Goswami,et al. Does half a pizza equal half a box of chocolates?: Proportional matching in an analogy task , 2001 .
[9] Anthony M. Norcia,et al. Development of Sensitivity to Texture and Contour Information in the Human Infant , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[10] Nora S. Newcombe,et al. Knowing Where Things Are in the Second Year of Life: Implications for Hippocampal Development , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] A. Ginsburg,et al. Infant visual preferences: a review and new theoretical treatment. , 1985, Advances in child development and behavior.
[12] Tim Hunter,et al. Young children's understanding of "more" and discrimination of number and surface area. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[13] E. N. Sokolov. Higher nervous functions; the orienting reflex. , 1963, Annual review of physiology.
[14] R. Church,et al. A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[15] Susan Carey,et al. Grey parrot number acquisition: The inference of cardinal value from ordinal position on the numeral list , 2012, Cognition.
[16] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Thinking about quantity: the intertwined development of spatial and numerical cognition. , 2015, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[17] M. Hershenson. VISUAL DISCRIMINATION IN THE HUMAN NEWBORN. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[18] C. Gallistel,et al. Representations in animal cognition: An introduction , 1990, Cognition.
[19] N. Newcombe,et al. Zooming in on spatial scaling: preschool children and adults use mental transformations to scale spaces. , 2014, Developmental psychology.
[20] Elizabeth S Spelke,et al. Neural signatures of number processing in human infants: evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition. , 2011, Developmental science.
[21] J. Huttenlocher,et al. The development of hierarchical representation of two-dimensional space. , 1996, Child development.
[22] Julie L. Booth,et al. Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.
[23] Michael Schneider,et al. Mental number line, number line estimation, and mathematical achievement : Their interrelations in grades 5 and 6 , 2009 .
[24] Michael Schneider,et al. Representations of the magnitudes of fractions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: is number one of them? , 2002, Psychological bulletin.
[26] E. Spelke,et al. Infants' Discrimination of Number vs. Continuous Extent , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[27] Victor A. F. Lamme,et al. Texture segregation is processed by primary visual cortex in man and monkey. Evidence from VEP experiments , 1992, Vision Research.
[28] Catherine Sophian,et al. How do people apprehend large numerosities? , 2008, Cognition.
[29] J. Gardner,et al. Arousal Effects on Visual Preferences in Neonates. , 1984 .
[30] Rochel Gelman,et al. Sometimes area counts more than number , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[31] Vincent Walsh,et al. Numerical Cognition: Reading Numbers from the Brain , 2009, Current Biology.
[32] C. Sophian,et al. When three is less than two: early developments in children's understanding of fractional quantities. , 1997, Developmental psychology.
[33] R K Clifton,et al. Heart-rate change as a component of the orienting response. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.
[34] N. Newcombe,et al. Development of mental transformation abilities , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[35] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children , 2009, Cognition.
[36] R. Sireteanu,et al. Texture segmentation and 'pop-out' in infants and children: the effect of test field size. , 1994, Spatial vision.
[37] R. Flom,et al. Relational congruence facilitates neural mapping of spatial and temporal magnitudes in preverbal infants , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[38] E. W. Ames,et al. Age Differences in Infants' Attention to Patterns of Different Complexities , 1966, Science.
[39] B. Karmel,et al. The effect of age, complexity, and amount of contour on pattern preferences in human infants. , 1969, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[40] M. Hershenson,et al. Preference for Shapes of Intermediate Variability in the Newborn Human , 1965, Science.
[41] D. Navon. Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.
[42] P. Gordon. Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.
[43] J. Piaget,et al. The Child's Conception of Number , 1953 .
[44] Philipp Sterzer,et al. Rapid Fear Detection Relies on High Spatial Frequencies , 2014, Psychological science.
[45] T. Wilcox,et al. Priming infants to attend to color and pattern information in an individuation task , 2004, Cognition.
[46] Kelly S. Mix. Children's equivalence judgments: Crossmapping effects , 2008 .
[47] A. J. Caron,et al. The effects of repeated exposure and stimulus complexity on visual fixation in infants , 1968 .
[48] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Minding the gap: Children's difficulty conceptualizing spatial intervals as linear measurement units. , 2015, Developmental psychology.
[49] R. T. Verrillo,et al. Specificity of a cutaneous receptor , 1966 .
[50] S. Luck,et al. The development of visual short-term memory capacity in infants. , 2003, Child development.
[51] A. Slater,et al. Visual Processing of Stimulus Compounds in Newborn Infants , 1991, Perception.
[52] Jae H. Paik,et al. U.S. and Korean children's comprehension of fraction names: a reexamination of cross-national differences. , 2003, Child development.
[53] Clarissa A. Thompson,et al. An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[54] J. Schweitzer,et al. Owner reports of attention, activity, and impulsivity in dogs: a replication study , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[55] H. Birch,et al. Infant visual fixation: the effect of concentricity, curvilinearity, and number of directions. , 1974, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[56] Zsuzsa Kaldy,et al. Identification of objects in 9‐month‐old infants: integrating ‘what’ and ‘where’ information , 2003 .
[57] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. The plasticity of near space: Evidence for contraction , 2009, Cognition.
[58] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.
[59] Tasha Posid,et al. Verbal Counting Moderates Perceptual Biases Found in Children's Cardinality Judgments , 2015 .
[60] Mark H. Johnson,et al. CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. , 1991, Psychological review.
[61] Frank H. Durgin,et al. Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. , 1995 .
[62] E. Spelke,et al. Numerical abstraction by human infants , 1990, Cognition.
[63] Yumiko Otsuka,et al. Temporal dynamics of spatial frequency processing in infants. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[64] Sean Duffy,et al. Developing symbolic capacity one step at a time , 2008, Cognition.
[65] B. Balas,et al. Infant Preference for Natural Texture Statistics is Modulated by Contrast Polarity. , 2014, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[66] P. Onghena,et al. The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year old children: evidence for a segmented linear model. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[67] Sean Duffy,et al. Developmental Changes in the Use of Absolute and Relative Information: The Case of Spatial Extent , 2007 .
[68] R. Siegler,et al. The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.
[69] Thomas R. Shultz,et al. Neural Network Simulation of Infant Familiarization to Artificial Sentences: Rule-Like Behavior Without Explicit Rules and Variables. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[70] A. Needham. Perceptual, Conceptual, and Representational Processes in Infancy. , 2001 .
[71] Clairice T. Veit,et al. Judgmental illusion produced by contrast with expectancy , 1973 .
[72] R. Held,et al. Development of Spatial Contrast Sensitivity from Infancy to Adulthood: Psychophysical Data , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.
[73] G. Mandler,et al. Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[74] D. Maurer,et al. Development of spatial and temporal vision during childhood , 1999, Vision Research.
[75] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants , 2010, Psychological science.
[76] M. W. Weir,et al. Infant's and young children's preference for complexity. , 1967, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[77] Susan C. Levine,et al. The Development of Proportional Reasoning: Effect of Continuous Versus Discrete Quantities , 2007 .
[78] H. Barth,et al. Developmental change in numerical estimation. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[79] Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al. Number without a language model , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[80] Linda B. Smith,et al. Signal clarity: an account of the variability in infant quantity discrimination tasks. , 2015, Developmental science.
[81] Barbara A. Shepperson,et al. Countable entities: Developmental changes , 1990, Cognition.
[82] Michael Bach,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of human texture segregation, an overview , 1998, Documenta Ophthalmologica.
[83] A. Henik,et al. Do subitizing deficits in developmental dyscalculia involve pattern recognition weakness? , 2013, Developmental science.
[84] S. Carey,et al. The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time , 2010, Cognition.
[85] Clarissa A. Thompson,et al. Costs and benefits of representational change: effects of context on age and sex differences in symbolic magnitude estimation. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[86] E. Spelke,et al. Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[87] Y. Gutfreund,et al. Saliency mapping in the optic tectum and its relationship to habituation , 2014, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[88] J. G. Wallace,et al. Cognitive development: An information-processing view , 1976 .
[89] PERCEPTION OF OBJECT ORIENTATION IN INFANTS1 , 2016 .
[90] J. Spencer,et al. Behavioral dynamics and neural grounding of a dynamic field theory of multi-object tracking. , 2012, Journal of integrative neuroscience.
[91] D. Greenberg,et al. Infancy and the optimal level of stimulation. , 1972, Child Development.
[92] S. Dehaene,et al. Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.
[93] R. Baillargeon,et al. Object Individuation in Infancy: The Use of Featural Information in Reasoning about Occlusion Events , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[94] Erin Ottmar,et al. Cutting In Line: Discontinuities in the Use of Large Numbers by Adults , 2014, CogSci.
[95] E. Spelke,et al. Number-Space Mapping in Human Infants , 2010, Psychological science.
[96] A. Oliva,et al. From Blobs to Boundary Edges: Evidence for Time- and Spatial-Scale-Dependent Scene Recognition , 1994 .
[97] E. Thelen,et al. Using dynamic field theory to rethink infant habituation. , 2006, Psychological review.
[98] E. Spelke,et al. Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.
[99] Paul Light,et al. Tools for Thought: The Measurement of Length and Area. , 1993 .
[100] L. Hedges,et al. Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.
[101] Linda B. Smith,et al. Grounding Word Learning in Space , 2011, PloS one.
[102] D. Berlyne. The influence of the albedo and complexity of stimuli on visual fixation in the human infant. , 1958, British journal of psychology.
[103] E. Spelke,et al. Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .
[104] M. Strauss,et al. Infant perception of numerosity. , 1981, Child development.
[105] Robert S. Siegler,et al. Representational change and children’s numerical estimation , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.
[106] Louise Kauffmann,et al. The neural bases of spatial frequency processing during scene perception , 2014, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[107] Sean Duffy,et al. Infants and Toddlers Discriminate Amount: Are They Measuring? , 2002, Psychological science.
[108] N. Newcombe,et al. Getting the big picture: Development of spatial scaling abilities , 2012 .
[109] P. Bryant,et al. Sharing and the understanding of number equivalence by young children , 1988 .
[110] J. Huttenlocher,et al. The Coding of Spatial Location in Young Children , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.
[111] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: A developmental study , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[112] T. Bower. Phenomenal identity and form perception in an infant , 1967 .
[113] Leslie B. Cohen,et al. Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences. , 1972, Child development.
[114] Nora S. Newcombe,et al. Infants' coding of location in continuous space. , 1999 .
[115] Linda B. Smith,et al. Open questions and a proposal: A critical review of the evidence on infant numerical abilities , 2013, Cognition.
[116] K. Fuson. Children's Counting and Concepts of Number , 1987 .
[117] G. N. Cantor,et al. Observing behavior in preschool children as a function of stimulus complexity. , 1963, Child development.
[118] Susan C. Levine,et al. Quantitative Development in Infancy and Early Childhood , 2002 .
[119] Arthur J. Baroody,et al. Meaningful Mathematics Instruction , 1991 .
[120] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Number Versus Contour Length in Infants' Discrimination of Small Visual Sets , 1999 .
[121] Steven J Luck,et al. Rapid Development of Feature Binding in Visual Short-Term Memory , 2006, Psychological science.
[122] I Abramov,et al. Eye Movement-Based Measures of Development of Spatial Contrast Sensitivity in Infants , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.
[123] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Do preschool children recognize auditory-visual numerical correspondences? , 1996, Child development.
[124] Justin Halberda,et al. Developmental change in the acuity of approximate number and area representations. , 2013, Developmental psychology.
[125] Sean Duffy,et al. It Is All Relative: How Young Children Encode Extent , 2005 .
[126] C. Agrillo. Numerical and Arithmetic Abilities in Non-primate Species , 2015 .
[127] Karen Wynn,et al. Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.
[128] Korbinian Moeller,et al. Children's early mental number line: logarithmic or decomposed linear? , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[129] Ellen Winner,et al. "Metaphorical" Mapping in Human Infants , 1981 .
[130] S. Wass. Comparing methods for measuring peak look duration: Are individual differences observed on screen-based tasks also found in more ecologically valid contexts? , 2014, Infant behavior & development.
[131] E. Spelke,et al. Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.
[132] Melissa M. Kibbe,et al. What Do Infants Remember When They Forget? Location and Identity in 6-Month-Olds’ Memory for Objects , 2011, Psychological science.
[133] Ty W. Boyer,et al. Child proportional scaling: is 1/3=2/6=3/9=4/12? , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[134] Marco Zorzi,et al. Emergence of a 'visual number sense' in hierarchical generative models , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[135] S. Carey,et al. Infants’ Metaphysics: The Case of Numerical Identity , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.
[136] Arthur J. Baroody,et al. Fostering Children's Mathematical Power: An Investigative Approach To K-8 Mathematics Instruction , 1998 .
[137] Lisa M. Oakes,et al. Using Habituation of Looking Time to Assess Mental Processes in Infancy , 2010, Journal of cognition and development : official journal of the Cognitive Development Society.
[138] G. Caputo,et al. Edge detection and surface `filling in' as shown by texture visual evoked potentials , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[139] W. Spears. ASSESSMENT OF VISUAL PREFERENCE AND DISCRIMINATION IN THE FOUR-MONTH-OLD INFANT. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[140] E. Spelke,et al. Detection of intermodal numerical correspondences by human infants. , 1983, Science.
[141] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Early fraction calculation ability. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[142] Miriam Bassok,et al. Magnitude comparison with different types of rational numbers. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[143] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Spatial Scaling in Young Children , 1999 .
[144] R L FANTZ,et al. The origin of form perception. , 1961, Scientific American.
[145] R. Lickliter,et al. Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. , 2000, Developmental psychology.
[146] E. Spelke,et al. Perception of objects and object boundaries by 3‐month‐old infants , 1987 .
[147] P. Bryant,et al. Children's Proportional Judgments: The Importance of “Half” , 1991 .
[148] B. Karmel,et al. Contour density and pattern configuration in visual preferences of infants , 1978 .
[149] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Discrimination of Large and Small Numerosities by Human Infants , 2004 .
[150] P. Starkey,et al. Perception of numbers by human infants. , 1980, Science.
[151] Janellen Huttenlocher,et al. Early development of scaling ability. , 2004, Developmental psychology.
[152] W. Gevers,et al. Topographic representation of high-level cognition: numerosity or sensory processing? , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[153] J. Fagan,et al. Visual attention to size and number of pattern details by term and preterm infants during the first six months. , 1975, Child development.
[154] Robert L. Goldstone,et al. The sensitization and differentiation of dimensions during category learning. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[155] Denis Mareschal,et al. Models of habituation in infancy , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[156] M. Haith,et al. The response of the human newborn to visual movement. , 1966, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[157] Scott P. Johnson,et al. Perception of object unity in 2-month-old infants. , 1995 .
[158] Christopher F. Sharpley,et al. Fraction Knowledge in Preschool Children. , 1988 .
[159] F. Attneave. Some informational aspects of visual perception. , 1954, Psychological review.
[160] A. Majid,et al. Prelinguistic Infants Are Sensitive to Space-Pitch Associations Found Across Cultures , 2014, Psychological science.
[161] B. Karmel. Complexity, amounts of contour, and visually dependent behavior in hooded rats, domestic chicks, and human infants. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[162] Hilary C Barth,et al. The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.
[163] Ty W. Boyer,et al. Prompting children to reason proportionally: Processing discrete units as continuous amounts. , 2015, Developmental psychology.
[164] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Do Korean Fraction Names Promote Part-Whole Reasoning? , 2008 .
[165] Janellen Huttenlocher,et al. Preexisting Knowledge Versus On-Line Learning , 2005, Psychological science.
[166] E. Spelke,et al. The construction of large number representations in adults , 2003, Cognition.
[167] Martin S. Banks,et al. Contrast sensitivity function of the infant visual system , 1976, Vision Research.
[168] T. Wilcox,et al. Infants' reasoning about opaque and transparent occluders in an individuation task , 2002, Cognition.
[169] Nora S Newcombe,et al. Thinking about quantity: the intertwined development of spatial and numerical cognition. , 2017, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[170] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Surface similarity and label knowledge impact early numerical comparisons , 2008 .
[171] Richard Lesh,et al. Rational number, ratio and proportion , 1992 .
[172] Mary K. Hoard,et al. Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. , 2007, Child development.
[173] L. Resnick,et al. Psychology of Mathematics for Instruction , 1981 .
[174] G Turkewitz,et al. Intersensory interaction in newborns: modification of visual preferences following exposure to sound. , 1981, Child development.
[175] J. Hegdé. Time course of visual perception: Coarse-to-fine processing and beyond , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.
[176] Marshall M. Haith,et al. Rules That Babies Look By: The Organization of Newborn Visual Activity , 1980 .
[177] Linda B. Smith,et al. Is there a developmental trend from integrality to separability in perception , 1978 .
[178] Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al. The Pen Is Mightier Than the Keyboard , 2014, Psychological science.
[179] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Similarity and Numerical Equivalence: Appearances Count , 1999 .
[180] G. Turpin,et al. Effects of stimulus intensity on autonomic responding: the problem of differentiating orienting and defense reflexes. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[181] A. Needham. Object recognition and object segregation in 4.5-month-old infants. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[182] Noam Chomsky,et al. वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .
[183] Kelly S. Mix,et al. How Spencer made number: first uses of the number words. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[184] R. Baillargeon,et al. Effects of prior experience on 4.5-month-old infants' object segregation , 1998 .